Document Detail

Vicarious learning from human models in monkeys.
Jump to Full Text
MedLine Citation:
PMID:  22768347     Owner:  NLM     Status:  MEDLINE    
Abstract/OtherAbstract:
We examined whether monkeys can learn by observing a human model, through vicarious learning. Two monkeys observed a human model demonstrating an object-reward association and consuming food found underneath an object. The monkeys observed human models as they solved more than 30 learning problems. For each problem, the human models made a choice between two objects, one of which concealed a piece of apple. In the test phase afterwards, the monkeys made a choice of their own. Learning was apparent from the first trial of the test phase, confirming the ability of monkeys to learn by vicarious observation of human models.
Authors:
Rossella Falcone; Emiliano Brunamonti; Aldo Genovesio
Related Documents :
19416467 - A study of a method for distribution analysis of skin color.
19288127 - Electro-physiological parameters of hepatic radiofrequency ablation--a comparison of an...
12551707 - Handling of computational in vitro/in vivo correlation problems by microsoft excel ii. ...
19937837 - Measurement and prediction of oral absorption.
3310997 - Artificial intelligence. expert systems for clinical diagnosis: are they worth the effort?
10877297 - Estimation of exploitation rate by combining mark-recapture procedures with a creel sur...
Publication Detail:
Type:  Journal Article; Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't     Date:  2012-07-02
Journal Detail:
Title:  PloS one     Volume:  7     ISSN:  1932-6203     ISO Abbreviation:  PLoS ONE     Publication Date:  2012  
Date Detail:
Created Date:  2012-07-06     Completed Date:  2012-11-13     Revised Date:  2013-07-12    
Medline Journal Info:
Nlm Unique ID:  101285081     Medline TA:  PLoS One     Country:  United States    
Other Details:
Languages:  eng     Pagination:  e40283     Citation Subset:  IM    
Affiliation:
Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy.
Export Citation:
APA/MLA Format     Download EndNote     Download BibTex
MeSH Terms
Descriptor/Qualifier:
Animals
Choice Behavior / physiology*
Humans
Macaca mulatta / physiology*
Models, Biological*
Problem Solving / physiology*
Comments/Corrections

From MEDLINE®/PubMed®, a database of the U.S. National Library of Medicine

Full Text
Journal Information
Journal ID (nlm-ta): PLoS One
Journal ID (iso-abbrev): PLoS ONE
Journal ID (publisher-id): plos
Journal ID (pmc): plosone
ISSN: 1932-6203
Publisher: Public Library of Science, San Francisco, USA
Article Information
Download PDF
Falcone et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Received Day: 21 Month: 3 Year: 2012
Accepted Day: 4 Month: 6 Year: 2012
collection publication date: Year: 2012
Electronic publication date: Day: 2 Month: 7 Year: 2012
Volume: 7 Issue: 7
E-location ID: e40283
ID: 3388051
PubMed Id: 22768347
Publisher Id: PONE-D-12-08728
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0040283

Vicarious Learning from Human Models in Monkeys Alternate Title:Observation Learning in Monkeys
Rossella Falconeaff1
Emiliano Brunamontiaff1
Aldo Genovesioaff1*
Gonzalo G. de Polaviejaedit1 Role: Editor
Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy
Cajal Institute, Consejo Superior de Investigaciones Científicas, Spain
Correspondence: * E-mail: aldo.genovesio@uniroma1.it
Contributed by footnote: Conceived and designed the experiments: AG RF. Performed the experiments: RF. Analyzed the data: AG RF EB. Wrote the paper: AG RF EB.

Introduction

Previous studies have shown that monkeys can learn and acquire motor behaviors by observation. Learning from observation can be very useful for primates, especially when used to learn where to find food and if it is safe and palatable, with the advantage of saving energy and decreasing the risk of harm [1], [2]. However, although several studies have provided evidence that monkeys can learn through the observation of a conspecific [3], [4], [5], [6], the evidence for learning from other primate species, especially humans, remains equivocal.

Research on mirror neurons has shown that monkeys have a shared neural representation of actions performed by human models or monkeys [7], [8], which suggests a neural substrate for learning through the observation of humans. But the ability of monkeys to learn through such observations has been denied [6], [9]. Menieur et al. [6] failed to show any improvement in performance when rhesus monkeys observed a human model performing a concurrent discrimination task and Brosnan and de Waal [9], likewise, reported that capuchin monkeys did not learn to associate tokens with high or low reward values when displayed to them by people.

One interpretation of these negative results is that monkeys might not monitor or pay attention to the humans' behavior as they do with a conspecific. Obviously, the extent to which monkeys attend to human observers and precisely what they attend to will have a crucial effect on what they learn. Flombaum and Santos [10] showed that monkeys are able to attribute significance to human perception, which shows that they attended to the pertinent information in that experiment. Our previous results also required sufficient attention [11]. We used a new nonmatch-to-goal (NMTG) task to test the monitoring abilities of macaque monkeys. This task required monkeys to monitor the human partner's goals. In some trials, after observing a human partner choose one goal, the monkeys were required to switch to a different goal in order to get a reward. We found that monkeys were able to perform this task, showing that in the circumstances of that experiment they could successfully monitor a human's behavior.

A failure in monitoring could explain the previous reports that deny observation learning by monkeys from humans. For example, in the study of Meunier et al. [6], the human models performed an object-discrimination task but did not consume a visible food reward. As a result, the observed events had less salience to a monkey than if food was consumed. The study of Brosnan and de Waal [9], likewise, did not involve the observation of food consumption. Accordingly, we tested whether introducing the vicarious consumption of food by a human model could promote learning in tasks involving the association between objects and rewards. We found that it did.


Materials and Methods
Animals

Animal care, housing and experimental procedures conformed to the European (Directive 86/609/ECC) and Italian (D.L. 116/92) laws on the use of nonhuman primates in scientific research. The research protocol was approved by the Italian Health Ministry (Central Direction for the Veterinary Service, approval number 199/2009-B). The housing conditions and the experimental procedures were in accordance with the European law on humane care and use of laboratory animals and complied with the recommendations of the Weatherall report (The use of non-human primates in research).

Both monkeys were monitored daily by the researchers, the animal care staff, and every other day by a veterinarian, to check the general conditions of health and welfare. To enrich their cognitive life, we routinely introduced in the home cage environment toys (often containing items of food that they liked) which promoted their exploratory behavior. Most of the time at the end of each experimental session, the researcher that tested the animals spent additional time interacting with the monkeys directly, giving them, for example, new objects to manipulate. This daily interaction with humans, in addition to the interaction that was part of the task performed, was intended to help the monkeys avoiding potential stress involved in the experiment. To increase the enrichment in the animal housing room, a monitor inside displayed motion pictures.

Behavioral testing

Two male rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta), 8.5 Kg and 5.5 Kg, respectively, participated in this study. They were on a controlled diet for the duration of the experiment.

The monkeys lived in a housing room and testing took place in a different room. During a typical testing session, the monkey sat in a primate chair and two humans, the experimenter and the model, stood nearby. The experimenter stood in front of the monkey, beyond his reach, displaying a metal tray equipped with two different objects. The model stood either on the left or on the right side of the monkey (Fig. 1a). A problem consisted of a pair of different objects varying in size, color, and texture–all emotionally neutral. For each pair, only one object in each pair, the positive object, concealed a piece of apple. Over a series of test sessions, spanning ten days, one monkey observed 33 problems and the other 34.

Testing for each pair of objects was divided in two phases: observation and test. In the observation phase, the human experimenter made six presentations of a pair of objects to the human model. These presentations took place within view, but out of reach, of the observer monkey. On all six of these trials, the human model grasped and elevated only the positive object and consumed the piece of apple beneath this object. After each trial, out of the monkey's view, the experimenter placed another piece of apple under the positive object and sometimes changed the position of the positive object, either to the same or to the alternative position on the tray, according to a pseudorandom schedule. The experimenter was careful not to give the monkey any cue as to where the reward was hidden, either by gazing or pointing at the positive object.

In the test phase, the monkey was tested with the same pair of objects used in the just-completed observation phase. The positive object was the same. The test phase consisted of a series of four trials (Fig. 1b). On each trial, the positive object's position was pseudorandomly assigned either to the monkey's left or right, and the monkey was allowed to displace only one of the two objects per trial. After each choice, a new trial followed.

After the test phase had been completed for a given pair of objects, a new pair of objects was used for the next observation phase, followed by another test phase, until the monkey had completed three to five sets of observation–test phases per day.


Results
Behavior during the observation phase

Both monkeys showed a similar behavior during the observation phase. They looked at the human model's actions while sitting very quietly at the beginning of a trial, when the model stood in front of the tray. Both monkeys became more active when they saw the model bringing the food to his mouth, occasionally extending their arms toward the human model, as if trying to obtain the food.

Behavior during the test phase

We calculated the performance of the monkeys on the first trial for all pairs of objects in the test phase. In monkey 1, the positive object in the first trial was presented 18/33 times to the monkey's right and 15/33 to the monkey's left in an unpredictable way. In monkey 2, the positive object was presented 16/34 times in the first trial to the monkey's right and 18/34 to the monkey's left, in a likewise unpredictable sequence.

Both monkeys performed above the chance level of 50% correct. Monkey 1 performed its first choice of 73% correct (24/33) and monkey 2 chose at 71% (24/34) correct. Both scores were significantly different than chance (binomial test, p<0.05) (Fig. 1c).

The first trial can only reflect the effect of observation; later trials could combine this knowledge with learning and exploratory behavior based on the results of the first trial. The performance for the second, third, and fourth trials were, respectively, 63% (20/32), 58% (19/33), 77% (24/31) for monkey 1, and 71% (24/34), 68% (23/34), and 74% (25/34) for monkey 2. Their performance was significantly different than chance (binomial test, p<0.05) in all but the third trials for both monkeys and the second trials for monkey 1.

We also calculated the performance for the second, third and fourth trial, sorting the trials based on the correctness of the first trial. After correct first choices, the percentage of correct choices for the second, third, and fourth trial was 78% (18/23), 79% (19/24), 92% (22/24) in monkey1 and 75% (18/24), 75% (18/24), 83% (20/24) in monkey 2, respectively.


Discussion

Using an object-reward learning paradigm, we showed that monkeys are able to display vicarious learning from a human model. Previous studies had suggested that monkeys could only learn vicariously from conspecifics [3], [6].

To date, several studies have investigated the ability of monkeys to interact socially with humans. Monkeys can copy some human facial movements, such as lips smacking and tongue protrusion even when they are infants [12], they can be taught to match the experimenter's hand gestures such as clapping hands [13], they can recognize when they are imitated by humans [14] and have an understanding of what humans see [10].

However, a recent study [6] reported that, although monkeys could learn stimulus-reward associations from conspecifics, they failed to learn from a human model. A similar result was found by Brosnan and de Waal [9] in capuchin monkeys, which failed to learn to associate tokens with either high-value or low-value foods by watching the experimenter holding up the token and the corresponding reward. The same monkeys could learn the food's value by watching a monkey model. Brosnan and de Waal [9] have suggested that social attachment and identification with a conspecific might be a requirement for social learning. Furthermore, Brosnan and de Waal [9] interpreted their negative results in support of the hypothesis that only a conspecific can enhance the salience of the objects required to learn. This hypothesis is in line with the identification-based observational learning model that emphasizes the emotional aspects of social learning [9].

However, both in the experiment of Brosnan et al. [9] and that of Meunier et al. [6] monkeys only observed a conspecific consuming the food. They did not watch the human models doing so. By introducing a vicarious reward, we showed that monkeys could learn through the observation of a human model. Therefore, vicarious rewards seem critical in promoting learning, their absence could explain the failures to learn from a human model in previous studies.

Observation learning dates to the Bandura's idea [15] that vicarious rewards enhance imitation because they informs the observer of the consequences of a behavior. Theorists have suggested that it could be important for cooperation and altruism [16]. Among other factors, the acquisition of reward could play the role of positive feedback regarding the success of the behavior [15], [17]. Vicarious reinforcement models support such a contribution, which can be understood as a form of Pavlovian conditioning (stimulus–outcome learning).

Vicarious learning has been contrasted with observational learning[18]. However, our experiment does not bear on that distinction because the monkeys were always rewarded for choosing the positive object, which was learned in the observational phase. Our results shows simply that monkeys can learn object–reward associations by observing these relationships as revealed by human models. In this situation, it seems necessary for the human model to consume the reward and not simply reveal it visually. However, for simpler behaviors, such as copying of left-right foraging choices in a testing apparatus, some learning can be observed even in the absence of a vicarious reward, at least among conspecifics [19].

In our study, as in others [5], [9], [6], the monkeys did not perform perfectly in the test phase. That could be because we offered only six observation trials for each problem. Future experiments should address whether monkeys can reach near-perfect levels of first-trial performance by increasing the amount of time and number of trials dedicated to observation. The improved performance after initial correct trials, in monkey 1, is suggestive of a learning process that continues after the observation phase. The other monkey instead maintained a similar high level of performance in the test trials.

Our study also points to the feasibility of neurophysiological investigations by using human-monkey (H–M) interactive paradigms to investigate the neural correlates of observational learning. Obviously, if the previous negative results were accepted–and monkeys could only learn vicariously from conspecifics–H–M paradigms would be precluded for neurophysiology.

More generally, H–M paradigms can represent a complementary or alternative approach in neurophysiology to the monkey-monkey (M–M) paradigm that has gained prominence in recent years. Several researchers have begun to study social adaptation [20], vicarious rewards [21] and behaviors regarding self vs. others [22] in M–M interaction. The H–M paradigm offers the advantage of a more controlled manipulation of the model's behavior, which becomes an independent variable. The M–M paradigm, by contrast, involves two dependent variables. When adapted to a more controlled experimental setup, H–M paradigms could be adapted to investigate the neural correlates of observational learning at the single-cell level, studied up to now only in neuroimaging experiments [23], [24], [25]. Future studies could asses whether humans could act as models in a variety of learning tasks, such as the conditional motor learning and sequence learning. In conclusion, we have shown that monkeys can learn vicariously from humans and thus opened a the ground for a new line of research that could adopt a human-monkey paradigm for studying social cognition.


Notes

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Funding: This work was supported by the ‘Sapienza’ University of Rome, Ricerca dell'Ateneo Federato di Scienze delle Politiche Pubbliche e Sanitarie (Protocol number: P288/09). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

We thank Simone di Plinio for assistance in testing the two monkeys and Stefano Ferraina and Steven Wise for comments on an initial version of the manuscript.


References
1. Valone TJ,Templeton JJ. Year: 2002Public information for the assessment of quality: a widespread social phenomenon.Philos Trans35715491557
2. Snowdon CS,Boe CY. Year: 2003Communication about unpalatable foods in tamarins (Saguinus oedipus).J Comp Psychol11714214812856784
3. Darby CL,Riopelle AJ. Year: 1959Observational learning in the rhesus monkey.J Comp Physiol Psychol52949813641473
4. Myers WA. Year: 1970Observational learning in monkeys.J Exp Anal Behav1422523516811470
5. Subiaul F,Romansky K,Cantlon JF,Klein T,Terrace H. Year: 2007Cognitive imitation in 2-year-old children (Homo sapiens): a comparison with rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta).Anim Cogn1036937517287996
6. Meunier M,Monfardini E,Boussaoud D. Year: 2007Learning by observation in rhesus monkeys.Neurobiol Learn Mem8824324817572114
7. di Pellegrino G,Fadiga L,Fogassi L,Gallese V,Rizzolatti G. Year: 1992Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study.Experimental Brain Research91176180
8. Fogassi L,Ferrari PF,Gesierich B,Rozzi S,Chersi F,et al. Year: 2005Parietal lobe: from action organization to intention understanding.Science30866266715860620
9. Brosnan SF,de Waal FB. Year: 2004Socially learned preferences for differentially rewarded tokens in the brown capuchin monkey (Cebus apella).J Comp Psychol11813313915250800
10. Flombaum JI,Santos LR. Year: 2005Rhesus monkeys attribute perceptions to others.Current Biology1544745215753039
11. Falcone R,Brunamonti E,Ferraina S,Genovesio A. Year: 2012Monkeys monitor human goals in a nonmatch-to-goal interactive task.Plos One7e3220922384177
12. Ferrari PF,Visalberghi E,Paukner A,Fogassi L,Ruggiero A,et al. Year: 2006Neonatal imitation in rhesus macaques.PLoS Biol4e30216953662
13. Kumashiro M,Ishibashi H,Uchiyama Y,Itakura S,Murata A. Year: 2003Natural imitation induced by joint attention in Japanese monkeys.Int J Psychophysiol50819914511838
14. Paukner A,Borelli E,Visalberghi E,Anderson JR,Ferrari PF. Year: 2005Macaques (Macaca nemestrina) recognize when they are being imitated.Biol Lett121922217148171
15. Bandura A. Year: 1977Social learning theory.Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall
16. Fehr E,Fischbacher U. Year: 2003The nature of human altruism.Nature 425, 785–791
17. Miklosi A. Year: 1999The ethological analysis of imitation.Biol Rev7434737410466254
18. Heyes CM,Jaldow E,Dawson G. Year: 1993Observational extinction: observation of non-reinforced responding reduces resistance to extinction in rats.Anim Learn Behav21221225
19. Bonnie KE,de Waal FB. Year: 2007Copying without rewards: socially influenced foraging decisions among brown capuchin monkeys.Anim Cogn102839217929373
20. Fujii N,Hihara S,Iriki A. Year: 2007Dynamic social adaptation of motion-related neurons in primate parietal cortex.PLoS One2e39717460764
21. Chang SW,Winecoff AA,Platt ML. Year: 2011Vicarious reinforcement in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta).Front Neurosci52721516263
22. Yoshida K,Saito N,Iriki A,Isoda M. Year: 2011Representation of others' action by neurons in monkey medial frontal cortex.Curr Biol2124925321256015
23. Mobbs D,Yu R,Meyer M,Passamonti L,Seymour B,et al. Year: 2009A key role for similarity in vicarious reward.Science32490019443777
24. Burke CJ,Tobler PN,Baddeley M,Schultz W. Year: 2010Neural mechanisms of observational learning.Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A107144311443620660717
25. Monfardini E,Brovelli A,Boussaoud D,Takerkart S,Wicker B. Year: 2008I learned from what you did: Retrieving visuomotor associations learned by observation.Neuroimage421207121318588987

Figures

[Figure ID: pone-0040283-g001]
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0040283.g001.
Figure 1  Experimental design and behavioral results.

A. Testing setup. B. The monkey displacing the positive object and exposing the food in the test phase. C. Percentage of correct first trials in the test phase. D. Performance as a function of trial number during the test phase. Blue and brown lines, all trials for monkey 1 and monkey 2; green and violet lines, 2nd-4th trials after a correct first choice in monkey 1 and monkey 2.



Article Categories:
  • Research Article
Article Categories:
  • Biology
    • Evolutionary Biology
      • Animal Behavior
    • Neuroscience
      • Behavioral Neuroscience
      • Learning and Memory
Article Categories:
  • Medicine
    • Mental Health
      • Psychology
        • Cognitive Psychology
          • Learning
Article Categories:
  • Social and Behavioral Sciences
    • Psychology
      • Cognitive Psychology
        • Learning
      • Social Psychology


Previous Document:  Simulating food web dynamics along a gradient: quantifying human influence.
Next Document:  The power laws of violence against women: rescaling research and policies.