Document Detail

Usual energy intake mediates the relationship between food reinforcement and BMI.
Jump to Full Text
MedLine Citation:
PMID:  22245983     Owner:  NLM     Status:  MEDLINE    
Abstract/OtherAbstract:
The relative reinforcing value of food (RRV(food)) is positively associated with energy consumed and overweight status. One hypothesis relating these variables is that food reinforcement is related to BMI through usual energy intake. Using a sample of two hundred fifty-two adults of varying weight and BMI levels, results showed that usual energy intake mediated the relationship between RRV(food) and BMI (estimated indirect effect = 0.0027, bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals (CIs) 0.0002-0.0068, effect ratio = 0.34), controlling for age, sex, minority status, education, and reinforcing value of reading (RRV(reading)). Laboratory and usual energy intake were correlated (r = 0.24, P < 0.001), indicating that laboratory energy intake could provide an index of eating behavior in the natural environment. The mediational relationship observed suggests that increasing or decreasing food reinforcement could influence body weight by altering food consumption. Research is needed to develop methods of modifying RRV(food) to determine experimentally whether manipulating food reinforcement would result in changes in body weight.
Authors:
Leonard H Epstein; Katelyn A Carr; Henry Lin; Kelly D Fletcher; James N Roemmich
Related Documents :
11436253 - The psychotherapy of a male anorectic.
21999723 - The role of a pre-load beverage on gastric volume and food intake: comparison between n...
24291303 - Developmental programming of the hpa and hpg axes by early-life stress in male and fema...
Publication Detail:
Type:  Comparative Study; Journal Article; Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural     Date:  2012-01-13
Journal Detail:
Title:  Obesity (Silver Spring, Md.)     Volume:  20     ISSN:  1930-739X     ISO Abbreviation:  Obesity (Silver Spring)     Publication Date:  2012 Sep 
Date Detail:
Created Date:  2012-08-28     Completed Date:  2013-01-02     Revised Date:  2013-06-26    
Medline Journal Info:
Nlm Unique ID:  101264860     Medline TA:  Obesity (Silver Spring)     Country:  United States    
Other Details:
Languages:  eng     Pagination:  1815-9     Citation Subset:  IM    
Affiliation:
Department of Pediatrics, University at Buffalo School of Medicine and Biomedical Sciences, Buffalo, New York, USA. LHENET@acsu.buffalo.edu
Data Bank Information
Bank Name/Acc. No.:
ClinicalTrials.gov/NCT00962117
Export Citation:
APA/MLA Format     Download EndNote     Download BibTex
MeSH Terms
Descriptor/Qualifier:
Adult
Body Mass Index*
Diet Records
Eating / psychology*
Energy Intake*
Female
Food Preferences
Humans
Male
Overweight / metabolism,  psychology*
Questionnaires
Regression Analysis
Reinforcement (Psychology)*
United States
Grant Support
ID/Acronym/Agency:
R01 DA024883/DA/NIDA NIH HHS; R01DA024883/DA/NIDA NIH HHS
Comments/Corrections

From MEDLINE®/PubMed®, a database of the U.S. National Library of Medicine

Full Text
Journal Information
Journal ID (nlm-ta): Obesity (Silver Spring)
Journal ID (iso-abbrev): Obesity (Silver Spring)
ISSN: 1930-7381
ISSN: 1930-739X
Publisher: Nature Publishing Group
Article Information
Download PDF
Copyright © 2012 The Obesity Society
open-access:
Received Day: 09 Month: 06 Year: 2011
Accepted Day: 22 Month: 12 Year: 2011
Print publication date: Month: 09 Year: 2012
epreprint publication date: Day: 13 Month: 01 Year: 2012
Electronic publication date: Day: 23 Month: 02 Year: 2012
Volume: 20 Issue: 9
First Page: 1815 Last Page: 1819
ID: 3428606
PubMed Id: 22245983
Publisher Item Identifier: oby20122
DOI: 10.1038/oby.2012.2

Usual Energy Intake Mediates the Relationship Between Food Reinforcement and BMI Alternate Title:Behavior and Psychology
Leonard H. Epstein1*
Katelyn A. Carr1
Henry Lin1
Kelly D. Fletcher1
James N. Roemmich1
1Department of Pediatrics, University at Buffalo School of Medicine and Biomedical Sciences, Buffalo, New York, USA
*(LHENET@acsu.buffalo.edu)

Introduction

Food is a powerful primary reinforcer that motivates people to eat (1). The reinforcing value (or reinforcing efficacy) of food (RRVfood) is assessed by how hard participants will work to obtain food. Based on procedures used to establish the reinforcing efficacy of drugs of abuse (2,3,4), the work requirements to obtain food are progressively increased and the schedule breakpoint, or the highest level of responding met by the subject, provides an index of food reinforcement. Using these methods, RRVfood has been cross-sectionally (5,6) and prospectively (7) related to obesity. RRVfood has further been associated with energy intake, as persons who find food more reinforcing eat more food during ad libitum eating tasks (8,9).

One way in which RRVfood is associated with obesity may be through energy intake, as people gain weight because they are in positive energy balance and obese individuals often consume more energy than leaner counterparts. The primary goal of this study was to assess whether usual energy intake, as assessed by a validated food frequency questionnaire (10,11) mediates the relationship between RRVfood and BMI. A secondary goal was to assess the relationship between laboratory energy and macronutrient intake in comparison to usual energy and macronutrient intake. Research has shown that RRVfood is related to energy intake in the laboratory (8,9) as well as usual energy intake assessed by repeated 24-h dietary recalls (12).

The most commonly used method of mediation analysis is the causal steps approach described by Baron and Kenny (13,14). In this model, the role of the mediating variable (M) in the relationship between the dependent variable (X) and the independent variable (Y) is confirmed if (i): all three variables are significantly correlated and (ii) if the significant relationship between X and Y is reduced to a nonsignificant level by addition of M into the regression model. However, the causal steps approach has been criticized for low statistical power, for drawing conclusions based on the inference rather than the quantification of the indirect (mediating) effect and for inherent susceptibility to both type I and type II error (15). Several methodologists have recommended newer tests of mediation (15,16,17). We test whether usual energy intake mediates the relationship between food reinforcement and BMI using the approach proposed by Preacher and Hayes (18) based on the (i) existence of a total effect to be mediated (e.g., a significant relationship between RRVfood and BMI) and (ii) a statistically significant indirect effect in the direction predicted, determined by the product of the regression coefficients of the RRVfood → increased energy intake (X→M) and increased energy intake → increased BMI (M→Y) paths with confidence intervals (CIs) generated by nonparametric bootstrapping procedures.


Methods and Procedures
Participants

Full details of the study design and participant recruitment have been reported elsewhere and are summarized below (12). Two-hundred fifty-two participants (71 nonobese females, 70 nonobese males, 51 obese females, 60 obese males) visited the laboratory for two sessions: an ad libitum snack-eating task and a food reinforcement task. The sample included 252 of 273 participants who were studied, as participants who did not complete the Block Food Frequency Questionnaire (n = 9) or reported implausible energy intakes (<800 or >6,000 kcal/day, n = 12) (19) were excluded from the analyses. Exclusionary criteria included taking medications associated with loss of appetite, smoking, diabetes, diagnosis of an eating or psychiatric disorder, allergic to study foods, were currently dieting, and did not rate at least a moderate liking (≥4 on a 9-point Likert-type scale) for five out of the six study foods. Participants received a $50 gift certificate to local stores for completing the study. The study was approved by the University at Buffalo Health Sciences Institutional Review Board. Participant characteristics are shown in Table 1.

Procedures

Participants visited the laboratory for two sessions: an ad libitum snack-eating task and a food reinforcement task scheduled 2–3 weeks apart. After the first session, participants completed the self-administered version of the 2005 Block Food Frequency Questionnaire (11) at home and returned it before the second session. Both sessions were scheduled between the hours of 2:00 and 5:00 PM, during a normal period that individuals would consume additional calories outside of meal time (20). Participants were asked to refrain from consuming food or drinking beverages other than water for at least 3 h and to not consume the experimental foods at least 24 h before the test session. Upon initial arrival at the laboratory, participants gave their written informed consent and completed dietary recalls to ensure adherence to study protocol. Before each session, participants were then provided a choice of three isocaloric energy bar preloads (Clif Bar & Company; Berkeley, CA; 42 g, 150 kcal, 4 g fat, 23 g carbohydrates, 7 g protein) to minimize the effects of hunger on energy intake and food reinforcement. The preload was provided in both sessions to keep the experimental conditions across days as similar as possible, and inclusion of a standard preload increases the ability to show individual differences in food reinforcement (21). Demographic information, height and weight measurements and three dietary habits questionnaires were administered at the end of the ad libitum eating session.

Ad Libitum eating task. The ad libitum food consumption task was presented as a taste test (12). Participants were provided 210–305 kcal (42–60 g) servings of six palatable, high-energy dense snack foods (amount of food presented (g) and energy density (kcal/g) shown in parentheses): Wavy Lay's Potato Chips (57 g, 5.4); Cooler Ranch Doritos (56 g, 5.4); plain M&M's (60 g, 5.0);Twix (48 g, 5.0); Kit Kat (42 g, 5.0); and Butterfinger (57 g, 4.5). Water was provided ad libitum. Participants were told that they could consume as much or as little of the food that they wanted as long as they tasted each food so that they could accurately rate the food on pleasantness, sweetness, blandness, flavorfulness, and bitterness using 9-point Likert-type scales. Afterwards, participants were given several dietary habits questionnaires to complete. They were also told that the food would be discarded after the session and so they were free to continue eating. When participants indicated that they were finished, they were asked to identify their favorite food from among the six available and told that this would be the food used in the food reinforcement test session. The primary dependent variables were laboratory energy and macronutrient intake.

Food reinforcement task. Specifics of the food reinforcement task have been previously described (12). The task was implemented as a computer program in which participants could choose to work for food (RRVfood) or reading (RRVreading) on concurrent schedules of reinforcement. Subjects responded by clicking a mouse button. Participants earned a point by meeting the schedule requirement, and they received a 70–101 kcal (14–20 g) portion of his or her preferred snack food selected during the ad libitum eating session or 2 min of time to spend reading for every five points earned, depending on which reward they were working for. Progressive fixed-ratio schedules were programmed for food and reading, with response requirements of 4, 8, 16, 32, 64, 128, 256, 512 and so forth for each point. Water was provided ad libitum. The dependent measure that we used was the breakpoint, or Pmax (2), which is the schedule (i.e., 4, 8, 16, 32, etc.) at which subjects last met response requirements for access to the food or non-food alternative. The test–retest reliability of RRVfood has been demonstrated (8), and RRVfood has been positively associated with energy intake in the ad libitum eating task (8,9) and with weight status (5,6,8).

Usual dietary intake. The self-administered version of the 2005 Block Food Frequency Questionnaire (Nutrition Quest, Berkeley, CA) was used to measure usual dietary intake. This 110-item questionnaire was designed to estimate usual and customary intake of a wide array of nutrients and food groups. Methodology for questionnaire development (10,22) and validation (11,23) are available. The food list for the 2005 revision was developed from NHANES 1999–2002 dietary recall data, and the nutrient database for that revision was developed from the USDA Food and Nutrient Database for Dietary Studies (FNDDS), version 1.0. Participants are asked to complete all questions including how frequently they consume specific foods and the usual portion size. The primary dependent variables were usual energy and macronutrient intake.

Height and weight. The participant's weight and height were measured using a digital scale (TANITA, Arlington Heights, IL) and a digital stadiometer (Measurement Concepts & Quick Medical, North Bend, WA). On the basis of height and weight data, BMI was calculated according to the following formula: BMI = kg/m2.

Food liking and hunger. Subjective ratings of hunger and food hedonics were collected before and after consumption of the preload using Likert-type scales. For hunger, 1 indicated not at all hungry/not at all full and 10 indicated extremely hungry/extremely full, while for hedonics 1 indicated not liking at all and 9 indicated liking very much.

Analytic plan

The goals of the analyses were to assess whether RRVfood is related to BMI, and whether usual energy intake mediates this relationship. Mediation models were established using multiple regression, controlling for age, sex, education, minority status, and RRVreading as covariates. The relationship between RRVfood and BMI was first evaluated. If there were a significant total effect of RRVfood on BMI, the size and significance of the indirect effect of RRVfood on BMI through energy intake was then estimated by the product of the regression coefficients of the predictive variables from the RRVfood → energy intake and the energy intake → BMI paths. CIs were constructed from 10,000 bootstrap resamples of the data (of the same size as the original study population, with replacement) and implemented via a macro developed by Preacher and Hayes (18). We present three ways to interpret the magnitude of the meditational effect. First, the mediator is considered to be significant if the indirect effect of RRVfood on BMI through energy intake is significantly different from 0 (the bootstrapped 95% CI does not contain 0). Second, the magnitude of the indirect effect reflects the change in the dependent variable (BMI) indirectly through the mediator variable (energy intake) per a unit change of the independent variable (RRVfood) (15). Third, the percent of the total effect of RRVfood on BMI explained by the indirect effect of RRVfood on BMI through energy intake was quantified by calculation of the effect ratio (indirect effect divided by the total effect) (24). Energy intake and dietary composition in the laboratory was compared to corresponding measures in the natural environment using Pearson product-moment correlations. Data were analyzed using SYSTAT 11 (Systat Software, Chicago, IL; 2004) and SAS 9.2 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC; 2008).


Results

Characteristics of the sample are presented in Table 1. RRVfood was related to BMI, (b = 0.0079, P = 0.031, n = 252), indicating the presence of a significant total effect. The indirect effect of RRVfood on BMI through usual energy intake was significant (estimate = 0.0027, 95% CI = 0.0002, 0.0068), suggesting that usual energy intake is a mediator of the relationship between RRVfood and BMI. The size of the indirect effect on BMI mediated through usual energy intake can be estimated by comparing BMI at the average breakpoint of responding for food to BMI at a one-s.d. increase in the breakpoint of responding for food, which would predict an increase of 0.36 BMI units. Based on the effect ratio, usual energy intake explained 34% of the association between RRVfood and BMI.

RRVfood was related to laboratory (r = 0.31, P <0.001) as well as usual energy intake (r = 0.35, P <0.001). Usual and laboratory energy intake were significantly related (r = 0.24, P < 0.001). Laboratory and usual consumption of protein (r = 0.23, P < 0.001), fat (r = 0.25, P < 0.001), and carbohydrates (r = 0.20, P = 0.002) were also significantly related.


Discussion

This study provides support for the hypothesis that energy intake mediates the relationship between food reinforcement and BMI. These results integrate previous findings showing that food reinforcement is related to body weight (6,8) and that food reinforcement is related to energy intake (8,9) to provide insight into the relationship between behavioral phenotypes, dietary behaviors, and obesity. The relationship observed suggests that modifying food reinforcement could influence body weight by altering energy intake. This is very relevant for weight control, as obese persons may find food more reinforcing than other behaviors and thus overeat, contributing to positive energy balance (1). It is also possible that the relationship could be extended towards understanding how to increase body weight in malnourished individuals. One factor that may result in the low body weight of these people is the reduced motivation to eat, and thus increasing the RRVfood may be a reasonable goal for this population. There are a wide number of conditions in which increasing energy intake is important for recovery. For example, increasing food reinforcement may be relevant for children with cystic fibrosis, who need to increase their energy intake to gain weight or improve intake of nutrients. Similarly, patients experiencing cachexia from a variety of illnesses may benefit from interventions that increase food reinforcement to enhance eating. The majority of interest in food reinforcement has been focused on obesity and reducing the RRVfood, but interventions that increase the RRVfood might be useful as an adjunctive treatment for a variety of diseases.

Given that food reinforcement may play a central role in the regulation of body weight through energy intake, innovative methods of modifying the RRVfood are warranted. These approaches may broadly fall into two categories: (i) the direct modification of food reinforcement or (ii) the indirect modification of food reinforcement by increasing the efficacy of non-food reinforcers or putting constraints on access to food reinforcers (1). For example, research suggests that the RRVfood can be sensitized, or increased, just as the motivation to self-administer drugs can be increased over repeated presentations (25,26). Temple and colleagues have shown that the RRVfood can be sensitized based on the characteristics of the food, the amount of food consumed, and the pattern of food consumption. Sensitization of food reinforcement is related to weight gain (27), and these effects may be moderated by weight status, as obese participants were more likely to show sensitization of food reinforcement than their leaner peers (27,28,29). An alternative is to indirectly modify food reinforcement by providing strong alternative reinforcers or by increasing the constraints on access to food. In scenarios where people have to decide between eating or engaging in alternative non-eating behaviors, such as many snacking opportunities, providing an alternative that is reinforcing and incompatible with eating may shift choice away from food, reducing energy intake (5,30). Similarly, increasing constraints on access to food reinforcers may shift choice from food to alternatives (30,31). If access between alternatives is equal, people generally choose the more reinforcing alternative (1). However, if access varies, choice may shift towards the commodity that is easier to obtain (1). These strategies could further be integrative and specifically target certain types of food. Increasing the variety of fruits and vegetables while restricting the variety of less healthy snack foods may shift preferences toward the former. Subsidizing healthy foods while taxing less healthy foods may similarly influence food selection (32).

The relationship between laboratory energy intake and usual energy intake, as well as the relationship between laboratory macronutrient intake and usual macronutrient intake, suggest that the ad libitum eating session may provide an index of eating in the natural environment. This would be a very useful addition to the tools available to assess energy intake, given challenges in measuring usual energy intake. It is recognized that self-reports of energy intake are compromised by underestimation (33,34). Treatment studies may benefit from including a standardized eating task as part of the outcome assessment. It might be useful to consider combining multiple measures of dietary intake to develop the most valid measure of eating, which could include laboratory eating, food frequency questionnaires, and dietary recalls.

There are several aspects of the study methods that may limit the generalization of the results. Dieters were excluded, since they may not want to eat in the laboratory and thus would not work for food. Given the challenges of measuring food reinforcement and ad libitum consumption of people who do not want to eat in the laboratory, it is unclear whether the results can be generalized to obese people who are dieting. In addition, all subjects were provided a preload before the food reinforcement and ad libitum snack-eating session to focus the task on hedonic, rather than homeostatic hunger (35). However, many eating situations occur when people are energy-deprived and research is needed to assess whether results obtained after a preload can be generalized to eating while experiencing homeostatic hunger. Finally, the current mediation analysis is based on cross-sectional data. A stronger test of whether energy intake mediates the effect of food reinforcement on body weight is through experimental analysis (16).

Food reinforcement may play a central role in the regulation of energy balance and of body weight, but experimental research investigating whether energy intake mediates the relationship between food reinforcement and body weight is needed. For example, a study could be designed to randomly assign one group of participants to an intervention that reduces food reinforcement and another group of participants to a control condition while tracking changes in energy intake and body weight. If decreased food reinforcement mediates the reduction in body weight through negative energy balance, then it would be expected that the groups would differ in body weight changes. Moreover, changes in food reinforcement and energy intake would be correlated in the experimental group but not in the control group. Such experiments would facilitate the development of new methods of modifying food reinforcement as well as confirm the mediating role of energy intake in the relationship between food reinforcement and body weight.


This trial was registered at http://www.clinicaltrials.gov as NCT00962117. Appreciation is expressed to Lora G. Roba, Vida Rostami, Lauren Angelucci, Nicole Gens, and Caitlin Hart for data collection and data entry and assisting in the implementation of protocol. This research was funded in part by a grant from the National Institute of Drug Abuse, R01DA024883 awarded to L.H.E.


Notes

The authors declared no conflict of interest.


REFERENCES
Epstein LH,Leddy JJ,Temple JL,Faith MS. Food reinforcement and eating: a multilevel analysisPsychol BullYear: 200713388490617723034
Bickel WK,Marsch LA,Carroll ME. Deconstructing relative reinforcing efficacy and situating the measures of pharmacological reinforcement with behavioral economics: a theoretical proposalPsychopharmacology (Berl)Year: 2000153445611255928
Richardson NR,Roberts DC. Progressive ratio schedules in drug self-administration studies in rats: a method to evaluate reinforcing efficacyJ Neurosci MethodsYear: 1996661118794935
Ator NA,Griffiths RR. Principles of drug abuse liability assessment in laboratory animalsDrug Alcohol DependYear: 200370S55S7212759197
Temple JL,Legierski CM,Giacomelli AM,Salvy SJ,Epstein LH. Overweight children find food more reinforcing and consume more energy than do nonoverweight childrenAm J Clin NutrYear: 2008871121112718469229
Saelens BE,Epstein LH. Reinforcing value of food in obese and non-obese womenAppetiteYear: 19962741508879418
Hill C,Saxton J,Webber L,Blundell J,Wardle J. The relative reinforcing value of food predicts weight gain in a longitudinal study of 7–10-y-old childrenAm J Clin NutrYear: 20099027628119535428
Epstein LH,Temple JL,Neaderhiser BJ. et al. Food reinforcement, the dopamine D2 receptor genotype, and energy intake in obese and nonobese humansBehav NeurosciYear: 200712187788617907820
Epstein LH,Wright SM,Paluch RA. et al. Food hedonics and reinforcement as determinants of laboratory food intake in smokersPhysiol BehavYear: 20048151151715135024
Block G,Hartman AM,Dresser CM. et al. A data-based approach to diet questionnaire design and testingAm J EpidemiolYear: 19861244534693740045
Block G,Woods M,Potosky A,Clifford C. Validation of a self-administered diet history questionnaire using multiple diet recordsJ Clin EpidemiolYear: 199043132713352254769
Epstein LH,Carr KA,Lin H,Fletcher KD. Food reinforcement, energy intake, and macronutrient choiceAm J Clin NutrYear: 201194121821543545
Baron RM,Kenny DA. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerationsJ Pers Soc PsycholYear: 198651117311823806354
Lockwood CM,DeFrancesco CA,Elliot DL,Beresford SA,Toobert DJ. Mediation analyses: applications in nutrition research and reading the literatureJ Am Diet AssocYear: 201011075376220430137
Preacher KJ,Kelley K. Effect size measures for mediation models: quantitative strategies for communicating indirect effectsPsychol MethodsYear: 2011169311521500915
Lockhart G,MacKinnon DP,Ohlrich V. Mediation analysis in psychosomatic medicine researchPsychosom MedYear: 201173294321148809
Hayes AF. Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation analysis in the new millenniumCommun MonogrYear: 200976408420
Preacher KJ,Hayes AF. SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation modelsBehav Res Methods Instrum ComputYear: 20043671773115641418
Schulze MB,Schulz M,Heidemann C. et al. Fiber and magnesium intake and incidence of type 2 diabetes: a prospective study and meta-analysisArch Intern MedYear: 200716795696517502538
Popkin BM,Duffey KJ. Does hunger and satiety drive eating anymore? Increasing eating occasions and decreasing time between eating occasions in the United StatesAm J Clin NutrYear: 2010911342134720237134
Reiss S,Havercamp S. The sensitivity theory of motivation: implications for psychopathologyBehav Res TherYear: 1996346216328870288
Block G,Coyle LM,Hartman AM,Scoppa SM. Revision of dietary analysis software for the Health Habits and History QuestionnaireAm J EpidemiolYear: 1994139119011968209877
Mares-Perlman JA,Klein BE,Klein R. et al. A diet history questionnaire ranks nutrient intakes in middle-aged and older men and women similarly to multiple food recordsJ NutrYear: 19931234895018463852
Shrout PE,Bolger N. Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: new procedures and recommendationsPsychol MethodsYear: 2002742244512530702
Robinson TE,Berridge KC. The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addictionBrain Res Brain Res RevYear: 1993182472918401595
Robinson TE,Berridge KC. The psychology and neurobiology of addiction: an incentive-sensitization viewAddictionYear: 200095Suppl 2S9111711002906
Temple JL,Epstein LH. Sensitization of food reinforcement is related to weight status and baseline food reinforcementInt J Obes (Lond)Year: 2011e-pub ahead of print 1 Nov 2011.
Clark EN,Dewey AM,Temple JL. Effects of daily snack food intake on food reinforcement depend on body mass index and energy densityAm J Clin NutrYear: 20109130030820016012
Temple JL,Bulkley AM,Badawy RL. et al. Differential effects of daily snack food intake on the reinforcing value of food in obese and nonobese womenAm J Clin NutrYear: 20099030431319458018
Goldfield GS,Epstein LH. Can fruits and vegetables and activities substitute for snack foodsHealth PsycholYear: 20022129930312027037
Lappalainen R,Epstein LH. A behavioral economics analysis of food choice in humansAppetiteYear: 19901481932337342
Epstein LH,Dearing KK,Roba LG,Finkelstein E. The influence of taxes and subsidies on energy purchased in an experimental purchasing studyPsychol SciYear: 20102140641420424078
Black AE,Goldberg GR,Jebb SA. et al. Critical evaluation of energy intake data using fundamental principles of energy physiology: 2. Evaluating the results of published surveysEur J Clin NutrYear: 1991455835991810720
Goldberg GR,Black AE,Jebb SA. et al. Critical evaluation of energy intake data using fundamental principles of energy physiology: 1. Derivation of cut-off limits to identify under-recordingEur J Clin NutrYear: 1991455695811810719
Lowe MR,Butryn ML. Hedonic hunger: a new dimension of appetitePhysiol BehavYear: 20079143243917531274

Article Categories:
  • Behavior and Psychology


Previous Document:  High expression of Mfn1 promotes early development of bovine SCNT embryos: Improvement of mitochondr...
Next Document:  In situ electroporation of surface-bound siRNAs in microwell arrays.