Document Detail

Resuscitation at birth and cognition at 8 years of age: a cohort study.
Jump to Full Text
MedLine Citation:
PMID:  19386357     Owner:  NLM     Status:  MEDLINE    
Abstract/OtherAbstract:
BACKGROUND: Mild cerebral injury might cause subtle defects in cognitive function that are only detectable as the child grows older. Our aim was to determine whether infants receiving resuscitation after birth, but with no symptoms of encephalopathy, have reduced intelligence quotient (IQ) scores in childhood.
METHODS: Three groups of infants were selected from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children: infants who were resuscitated at birth but were asymptomatic for encephalopathy and had no further neonatal care (n=815), those who were resuscitated and had neonatal care for symptoms of encephalopathy (n=58), and the reference group who were not resuscitated, were asymptomatic for encephalopathy, and had no further neonatal care (n=10 609). Cognitive function was assessed at a mean age of 8.6 years (SD 0.33); a low IQ score was defined as less than 80. IQ scores were obtained for 5953 children with a shortened version of the Weschler intelligence scale for children (WISC-III), the remaining 5529 were non-responders. All children did not complete all parts of the test, and therefore multiplied IQ values comparable to the full-scale test were only available for 5887 children. Results were adjusted for clinical and social covariates. Chained equations were used to impute missing values of covariates.
FINDINGS: In the main analysis at 8 years of age (n=5887), increased risk of a low IQ score was recorded in both resuscitated infants asymptomatic for encephalopathy (odds ratio 1.65 [95% CI 1.13-2.43]) and those with symptoms of encephalopathy (6.22 [1.57-24.65]). However, the population of asymptomatic infants was larger than that of infants with encephalopathy, and therefore the population attributable risk fraction for an IQ score that might be attributable to the need for resuscitation at birth was 3.4% (95% CI 0.5-6.3) for asymptomatic infants and 1.2% (0.2-2.2) for those who developed encephalopathy.
INTERPRETATION: Infants who were resuscitated had increased risk of a low IQ score, even if they remained healthy during the neonatal period. Resuscitated infants asymptomatic for encephalopathy might result in a larger proportion of adults with low IQs than do those who develop neurological symptoms consistent with encephalopathy.
FUNDING: Wellcome Trust.
Authors:
David E Odd; Glyn Lewis; Andrew Whitelaw; David Gunnell
Related Documents :
3303957 - La ferrassie 6 and the development of neandertal pubic morphology.
17575477 - Optimal oxygenation at birth and in the neonatal period.
21284327 - The relationship between abdominal aortic intima-media thickness and lipid profile in n...
18369237 - Occurrence of physical dormancy in seeds of australian sapindaceae: a survey of 14 spec...
8358917 - Sensitivity of manual palpation in testing the neonatal hip.
24441767 - Domain-specific effects of prenatal exposure to pcbs, mercury, and lead on infant cogni...
Publication Detail:
Type:  Journal Article; Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't     Date:  2009-04-20
Journal Detail:
Title:  Lancet     Volume:  373     ISSN:  1474-547X     ISO Abbreviation:  Lancet     Publication Date:  2009 May 
Date Detail:
Created Date:  2009-05-11     Completed Date:  2009-06-01     Revised Date:  2014-02-19    
Medline Journal Info:
Nlm Unique ID:  2985213R     Medline TA:  Lancet     Country:  England    
Other Details:
Languages:  eng     Pagination:  1615-22     Citation Subset:  AIM; IM    
Export Citation:
APA/MLA Format     Download EndNote     Download BibTex
MeSH Terms
Descriptor/Qualifier:
Apgar Score
Asphyxia Neonatorum* / complications,  therapy
Child
Cognition Disorders / diagnosis,  epidemiology,  etiology*
Cohort Studies
Developmental Disabilities / diagnosis,  epidemiology,  etiology*
England / epidemiology
Female
Humans
Hypoxia, Brain / complications,  therapy
Infant, Newborn
Intelligence Tests
Linear Models
Logistic Models
Male
Multivariate Analysis
Resuscitation* / adverse effects
Risk Factors
Sensitivity and Specificity
Severity of Illness Index
Socioeconomic Factors
Wechsler Scales
Young Adult
Grant Support
ID/Acronym/Agency:
077036//Wellcome Trust; G9815508//Medical Research Council; //Medical Research Council
Comments/Corrections
Comment In:
Lancet. 2009 Aug 1;374(9687):377-8   [PMID:  19647605 ]
Lancet. 2009 May 9;373(9675):1581-2   [PMID:  19386356 ]

From MEDLINE®/PubMed®, a database of the U.S. National Library of Medicine

Full Text
Journal Information
Journal ID (nlm-ta): Lancet
ISSN: 0140-6736
ISSN: 1474-547X
Publisher: Lancet Publishing Group
Article Information
Download PDF
© 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved..
License:This document may be redistributed and reused, subject to certain conditions.
Print publication date: Day: 9 Month: 5 Year: 2009
Volume: 373 Issue: 9675
First Page: 1615 Last Page: 1622
ID: 2688587
PubMed Id: 19386357
Publisher Id: LANCET60244
DOI: 10.1016/S0140-6736(09)60244-0

Resuscitation at birth and cognition at 8 years of age: a cohort study
David E Odda* Email: david.odd@nbt.nhs.uk
Glyn Lewisb
Andrew Whitelawa
David Gunnellc
aClinical Science at North Bristol, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
bDepartment of Community Based Medicine, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
cDepartment of Social Medicine, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
*Correspondence to: Dr David Odd, Neonatal Intensive Care Unit, Southmead Hospital, Bristol BS10 5NB, UK david.odd@nbt.nhs.uk

Introduction

Mild degrees of hypoxia or other damaging processes during birth might cause impaired cognitive function in later life. The theory of a continuum of reproductive casualty1 suggests that although severe perinatal cerebral events cause death or obvious neurological deficit, mild events might cause subtle defects in cognitive function that are only detectable as the child grows older. Clinically important brain damage is widely believed to occur only if the hypoxic injury causes encephalopathy during the neonatal period,2,3 but evidence has been difficult to obtain. Even in infants with encephalopathy, the profile of cognitive effect is unclear. Since mild perinatal hypoxic events are more frequent than severe events, the long-term effect on the population could be substantial.

We recently reported a reduced intelligence quotient (IQ) in men at about 18 years of age, who had a low Apgar score at birth but did not develop encephalopathy.4 Previous small studies might not have had sufficient statistical power to detect such small differences in IQ.5,6 The Apgar score is commonly used to measure condition at birth, but the test is often criticised for being subjective and poorly reproducible.7 By contrast, the need for active resuscitation is a specific sign of delayed onset of respiration and could therefore indicate recent cerebral injury, especially hypoxia–ischaemia. In animal models, substantial fetal hypoxia was followed by delayed onset of respiration after birth,8 although other causes of poor condition at birth are infection,9 pre-existing fetal disorders,10 and congenital anomalies.11,12

We aimed to investigate whether childhood IQ score is affected by physiological compromise of a severity to warrant resuscitation during birth in infants who do or do not subsequently develop encephalopathy.


Methods
Patients

11 981 singleton infants, born at or after 36 weeks' gestation at two major maternity units in the study area, and alive at 8 years of age (figure), were selected from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC). The ALSPAC cohort consists of more than 80% of children who were born in Bristol, UK, between April 1, 1991, and Dec 31, 1992, and contains data for 14 688 infants. Data for cohort members and their families are regularly obtained from self-completed questionnaires or half-day clinics, or are retrieved from routine medical or educational records.

499 infants were excluded from the study because resuscitation data were missing or they needed neonatal care but were not resuscitated or had no symptoms of encephalopathy (figure). Three groups were defined from the remaining 11 482 infants (figure): those who were resuscitated but were asymptomatic for encephalopathy and had no further neonatal care (n=815), those who were resuscitated and needed neonatal care for symptoms of encephalopathy (n=58), and the reference group of healthy infants who were not resuscitated, were asymptomatic for encephalopathy, and had no further neonatal care (n=10 609).

Ethical approval for the study was given by the ALSPAC law and ethics committee and the local research ethics committees. One parent of every child signed consent for enrolment of the child in to the ALSPAC study.

Procedures

Information about resuscitation and perinatal health was retrieved from computer records for 12 799 infants (87% of the cohort) born at two major maternity units. We deemed infants to have evidence of physiological compromise if they required either positive pressure respiratory support (face mask or endotrachael tube) or cardiac compressions at birth. Seizures, jitteriness, high-pitched cry, hypotonia or hypertonia, or hyper-reflexia were regarded as symptoms of encephalopathy. Although disturbances of consciousness were not prospectively recorded, seizures and disturbances in tone are central features for clinical diagnosis of neonatal encephalopathy.13

Cognitive function was assessed at a half-day clinic when study participants were a mean age of 8·6 years (SD 0·33); IQ data was gathered from 5953 children. Parents and children were initially invited to attend by letter; if there was no response, another letter was followed by telephone or personal contact, and a final letter was sent after 3 months of no response. IQ was assessed with the most recent version of the Weschler intelligence scale for children at that time—WISC-III—which is divided into verbal and performance subtests (panel).14

The same shortened version of the test was administered to all children, but even though the test was shortened, the number of children that completed each question within the verbal and performance subtests differed because children became tired, frustrated, or began to lose concentration after different time periods (panel). Alternate questions were asked for every subtest, except for the coding subtest, to shorten the test. Raw scores from every subtest were multiplied to predict scores comparable with the full version of the test. Tables from the WISC manual were used to calculate verbal, performance, and full-scale IQ scores. For consistency with our previous work,4 a low IQ score was defined as less than 80.15

Potential confounders and modifying variables for the statistical analysis of the infants fell into perinatal and social categories. Perinatal factors were sex, parity, gestational age, birthweight, length and head circumference, method of birth, maternal hypertension, and neonatal sepsis. Social factors were maternal age, socioeconomic group,16 education, and car ownership, and housing tenure, crowding index (number of household members per room), and ethnicity.

Statistical analysis

Primary analyses investigated the association between resuscitation and a low score in the summary IQ variables (verbal, performance, and full-scale IQ). We used logistic regression models, with an IQ score of less than 80 as the binary outcome, and linear regression models, with IQ score as a continuous variable. Secondary analyses with the ten verbal and performance subtests (Panel), and the verbal and performance IQ tests as outcomes were used to further investigate the association. To adjust for possible confounders, the perinatal and social modifying variables were added to the model, in blocks of common variables (eg, socioeconomic factors). We assessed whether maternal occupation, education, and sex modified the association of resuscitation status with cognition by fitting appropriate interaction terms to the model. Ordinal variables were tested for linearity and were included in the model as linear terms if appropriate. The population attributable risk factor was calculated from the final logistic regression model.17

Since all children did not complete all parts of the IQ test, each analysis contained a slightly different number of children (figure): 5398 children had complete IQ data, 555 had incomplete data, and 5529 had no data. Our main analyses used all available data. For example, 5887 children supplied full-scale IQ data but only 4857 children had complete data on all exposure (resuscitation status), outcome (IQ data), and confounder variables. To reduce selection bias, the chained equations missing data method was used to impute the missing confounder values only (exposure or outcome data was not imputed).18 We were therefore able to report analyses for the same number of children for both crude and adjusted analyses.

We did sensitivity analyses to investigate the effect of these missing data by imputing all variables including the outcome but not exposure data (n=11 482) and by use of a complete case-only analysis (n=4857), which restricted the analysis to participants with complete data about all exposure, outcome, and confounder variables.

We did two further sensitivity analyses. First, excluding infants who developed cerebral palsy (n=13), and second with the cutpoint for a low IQ score of less than 70. This second cutpoint identified infants with IQ scores about 2 SD from the mean and is regarded as exceptionally low.15 We also derived the association between a low Apgar score (less than 7) at 1 and 5 min of age, and the risk of a low IQ score in asymptomatic infants. We chose not to use Bonferonni corrections to adjust our p values for the multiple statistical tests done, because the IQ scores were strongly intercorrelated or interdependent, and therefore such an approach would be overly conservative. All analyses used Stata (version 9), and a two-tailed p value of less than 0·05 was regarded as statistically significant.

Role of the funding source

The funding organisation did not participate in study design, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation, writing of the report, or the decision to submit the paper for publication. The corresponding author had full access to all the data in the study and had final responsibility for the decision to submit for publication.


Results

Characteristics of 11 482 eligible infants (table 1) showed no substantial difference in socioeconomic group or maternal age between resuscitated and non-resuscitated infants. Mothers with infants who were resuscitated and were asymptomatic for encephalopathy were the least educated, followed by those in the reference group. First pregnancy and maternal hypertension were more common in resuscitated infants with encephalopathy than asymptomatic infants or the reference group. Maternal pyrexia during labour and caesarean section was more common in both resuscitated groups than in the reference group. Head circumference was slightly increased and both birthweight and the Apgar score were slightly decreased in resuscitated infants. Table 2 shows Apgar and resuscitation status in the three groups. A higher proportion of infants who were resuscitated and developed symptoms of encephalopathy went on develop cerebral palsy than was seen in the asymptomatic and reference groups (table 1).

To assess selection bias, we compared the characteristics of the 5529 infants who had no IQ data with the 5953 infants who were included in the main analyses. Infants who had no IQ data were more likely than were those with at least some IQ data to have needed resuscitation, but the association was not significant (8·5% vs 7·6%, p=0·069). These infants had identical median Apgar scores at 1 min (9 [IQR 8–9] vs 9 [8–9]; p=0·70) and 5 min (10 [9–10] vs 10 [9–10]; p=0·31) after birth.

The mean IQ scores (table 3) showed a strong association of the performance IQ and full-scale IQ, and a non-significant association of the verbal IQ, with the condition of the infant at birth. Of the infants with low IQ scores (table 4), performance IQ and full-scale IQ were associated with resuscitation status, but verbal IQ was non-significantly associated.

In the final logistic regression model (table 5) the need for resuscitation in asymptomatic infants was associated with a low full-scale IQ score, although there was little evidence for a specific association with either a low verbal or performance IQ separately. Infants who developed encephalopathy after resuscitation had an increased risk of a low performance and full-scale IQ, although the association with verbal IQ was not significant. The absolute risk of a low IQ score was 6·5% for reference group infants, 9·8% (risk difference 3·3% [95% CI 0·3–6·2]) for asymptomatic resuscitated infants, and 23·1% (risk difference 16·6% [0·4–32·8]) for resuscitated infants with encephalopathy.

The population attributable risk fraction was used to assess the effect of resuscitation status on IQ score. The proportion of individuals with low IQ scores that might be attributable to the need for resuscitation at birth was 3·4% (95% CI 0·5–6·3) for asymptomatic infants and 1·2% (0·2–2·2) for those who developed symptoms of encephalopathy. For both the linear and logistic regression models, adjustment for confounding factors did not appreciably modify the associations between resuscitation and IQ scores. There was little evidence that the association between resuscitation status and IQ differed by sex (pinteraction=0·33), maternal education (pinteraction=0·65), or maternal socioeconomic group (pinteraction=0·72).

In the fully adjusted linear-regression analysis (table 6) the mean verbal, performance, and full-scale IQ scores were lower, but not significantly so, in asymptomatic resuscitated infants than were those in the reference group. Conversely, verbal, performance, and full-scale IQ scores were all significantly lower in infants who developed encephalopathy after resuscitation than in the reference group. In the IQ subtests, asymptomatic resuscitated infants had lower similarities scores than did the reference group. Resuscitated infants who developed encephalopathy had decreased vocabulary, comprehension, and object assembly scores, and there was non-significant evidence of reduced information and coding scores.

Complete case analysis of only infants with complete data (n=4857) showed strengthened, but less precise, associations between resuscitation status and a low IQ score from odds ratios: asymptomatic infants 1·78 (95% CI 1·16–2·72), p=0·008; infants with encephalopathy 11·36 (2·65–48·80), p=0·001. Repeat of the analysis with imputation of covariates and IQ data (n=11 482) showed some evidence of an association: asymptomatic infants 1·44 (1·03–2·02), p=0·033; infants with encephalopathy 3·59 (1·01–12·82), p=0·049. Full results from these other models are available from the authors on request.

In further sensitivity analyses, we repeated the analysis with the low IQ score cutpoint of 70, which provided results consistent with the main analysis, although, reduction in the absolute number of infants with low scores resulted in widened confidence intervals: asymptomatic infants 1·91 (0·97–3·75), p=0·060; infants with encephalopathy 8·22 (0·94–72·04), p=0·057. The analysis was also repeated with exclusion of infants who developed cerebral palsy and showed little effect on the association between resuscitation and low IQ score for asymptomatic infants (1·64 [1·12–2·40], p=0·012), but the association for infants with encephalopathy was not significant (3·69 [0·73–18·52], p=0·113). The profile of cognitive changes remained similar (data not shown). With an Apgar score of less than 7, the association between resuscitation and low IQ score for asymptomatic infants at 5 min of age was 1·34 (0·42–4·27), p=0·662.


Discussion

The profile of infant characteristics suggested that associations between resuscitation status and perinatal or social factors were consistent with the findings of previous studies.11,12 Resuscitation was associated with non-white ethnicity and poor maternal education. Infants with reduced birthweight, from primiparous mothers, or born by caesarean section or instrumental delivery also had increased risk of needing resuscitation at birth. We showed an association between resuscitation at birth and cognitive functioning at 8 years of age. Infants who needed resuscitation, even if they did not develop symptoms of encephalopathy in the neonatal period, had a substantially increased risk of a low full-scale IQ score. Infants who developed signs of encephalopathy after resuscitation showed about a five-times increase in low performance and full-scale IQ scores compared with the reference group.

The data suggest that mild perinatal physiological compromise might be sufficient to cause subtle neuronal or synaptic damage, and thereby affect cognition in childhood and potentially in adulthood. By comparison, substantial perinatal compromise presents with neonatal encephalopathy and large cognitive deficits. The study had restricted power to detect differences in individual IQ subtests, and deficits in cognitive function are likely to present with reduced scores in several overlapping tests. Consequently interpretation of the subtests in isolation should be done with caution. Myers'8 studies of animal models suggest that prolonged hypoxia might affect vascular watershed areas (between two major cerebral arteries) of the brain—namely, the frontal, parietal, and occipital cortex—whereas acute anoxic events might preferentially affect deeper structures—eg, the basal ganglia and brain stem. The results of this study are consistent with prolonged partial hypoxia.

Kaufman19 reports that resuscitated infants have reduced scores in vocabulary and comprehension tests, specifically single-word comprehension and logical abstract thinking, which are measures of verbal conceptualisation ability. These functions have been localised to the prefrontal cortex,20,21 and therefore reduced function is consistent with a watershed injury to the brain in both groups of infants in our study. By contrast, infants with severe physiological compromise in our study also had substantially reduced scores for visual-perceptual abilities and spatial skills, which could indicate injury to deeper structures such as the thalamus.22

Resuscitation was strongly associated with an increased risk of a low IQ score, but the association with the mean reduction in IQ was not significant, which could be a chance finding. However, this result could also be a biologically plausible outcome from physiological compromise at birth—eg, only a subgroup of infants would have experienced sufficient compromise to damage the relevant brain regions and impair cognitive performance. The fact that the neonatal brain can repair itself also suggests that many neonates would not show deficits in cognitive function during childhood and adulthood unless the injury at birth was sufficiently severe. Our results did show a small reduction in the mean IQ score in the asymptomatic group, which is consistent with the findings in our previous study.4

ALSPAC is a large cohort of a population broadly representative of those living in the rest of the UK,23,24 but we had restricted statistical power to detect robust associations in infants with probable hypoxic–ischaemic encephalopathy since the condition is rare (0·5%), and consequently the confidence intervals were wide. The proportion of infants who received resuscitation in this cohort is consistent with published work, in which the rate varies between 1%25 and 14%.26 This variation might be caused by the clinician's subjective decision to resuscitate, despite clear guidelines,27 which will result in some error of measurement. Since 5529 (48%) of the eligible infants did not attend the IQ testing session, a missing data method was used to investigate potential selection bias. We repeated the analysis with the fully imputed data (n=11 482) and complete-case only (n=4857) analyses to assess the effect of missing data and reached similar conclusions. Some infants who had neonatal encephalopathy might not have been correctly identified, either because of absence of resuscitation details, or by having unreported symptoms of encephalopathy. Whether this absence of data could cause important selection bias is unclear, but repetition of the analysis with all infants admitted for neonatal care after resuscitation showed similar results.

Our findings could also be explained by prenatal disorders causing poor birth condition and predisposition to cognitive dysfunction in later life. We were able to control for several common prenatal disorders (eg, sepsis, prematurity); we could not control for rare abnormalities (eg, neuromuscular disorders), but they are less likely to affect the recorded associations. We attempted to control for important confounders but some degree of residual confounding is possible. Additionally, although we were able to correct for several socioeconomic factors, we have not adjusted for antenatal alcohol consumption or smoking exposure, and therefore any possible confounding effect of these factors has not been corrected for.

Use of 100% oxygen at birth is associated with worse outcomes than the use of room air in infants who are resuscitated,28,29 possibly because of free-radical injury and pulmonary atelectasis. Infants in this study were born in the early 1990s when 100% oxygen was the only gas connected to resuscitation equipment in the study centres. Therefore resuscitated infants were probably exposed to high partial pressures of oxygen for longer periods than infants would be today. The proportion of the association between resuscitation and IQ that might be attributable to hyperoxic damage, secondary to the resuscitation process itself, is unclear. Therefore, our findings might not be applicable to infants born in the present day.

Population studies are valuable to assess the effect of hypoxia on cognitive function in childhood. Many of these studies use the Apgar score to measure hypoxia, but large interobserver variability has caused concern.7 Data from our study showed a reduction in IQ in asymptomatic infants with a low Apgar score. This result is consistent with our previous study,4 in which asymptomatic infants with an Apgar score below 7 at 5 min of age had an increased risk of a low IQ (n=176 524, odds ratio 1·35 [95% CI 1·07–1·69]). The IQ was measured as part of a compulsory examination at 18 years of age, and, consequently, direct comparison with the WISC-III at 8 years of age is difficult. Nevertheless, similar analysis with Apgar data gathered from ALSPAC participants provided a compatible, though less precise, result (n=6045, 1·34 [0·42–4·27]).

We are not aware of other studies that have shown an association between transient physiological compromise at birth and cognitive defects in subsequently healthy infants. Viggedal5 and Blackman6 and their colleagues have reported reduced cognition only in infants who developed symptoms of encephalopathy. However, these studies consisted of only 20 and 38 infants, respectively, and without signs of neurological function the statistical power was probably insufficient to detect the modest association noted in this study.

In our study, resuscitated infants with encephalopathy had a substantially increased risk of a low IQ score compared with both resuscitated infants who remained asymptomatic and the reference group. However, since asymptomatic infants were more common than those with encephalopathy, they would have a larger population effect if a causal relation exists between resuscitation and the risk of a low IQ score.

In the ALSPAC cohort, infants who received resuscitation but did not develop symptoms of encephalopathy had an increased risk of a low IQ score at age 8 years. If this were a causal relation, it could account for about one in 30 adults who have a low IQ, which is a larger proportion than is attributable to the infants who develop encephalopathy. These findings are consistent with a cerebral injury of sufficient severity to delay respiration, but insufficient to cause obvious symptoms of encephalopathy. Our results support the idea of a continuum of reproductive casualty.1 Education and training interventions targeted at obstetric and neonatal staff improve measures of birth condition,30 and the identified association could have important population benefits if it is shown to be causal. Physiological compromise before or during birth could damage areas of the brain that are critical for specific cognitive functions.

Contributors

All authors designed the study, analysed the data, wrote the manuscript, and commented on the final draft.

Conflicts of interest

We declare that we have no conflicts of interest.


Acknowledgments

We thank the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists, and nurses. This research was funded by a fellowship award (077036) to DO from the Wellcome Trust. The UK Medical Research Council, the Wellcome Trust, and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC.


References
1. Pasamanick B,Rogers ME,Lilienfeld AM. Pregnancy experience and the development of behavior disorders in childrenAm J Psychiatry 1956;112:613–618. [pmid: 13292548]
2. Committee on Obstetric Practice, ACOG; American Academy of Pediatrics; Committee on Fetus and Newborn, ACOGACOG Committee Opinion. Number 333, May 2006 (replaces No. 174, July 1996): the Apgar scoreObstet Gynecol 2006;107:1209–1212. [pmid: 16648434]
3. MacLennan A. A template for defining a causal relation between acute intrapartum events and cerebral palsy: international consensus statementBMJ 1999;319:1054–1059. [pmid: 10521205]
4. Odd DE,Rasmussen F,Gunnell D,Lewis G,Whitelaw A. A cohort study of low Apgar scores and cognitive outcomesArch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 2008;93:F115–F120. [pmid: 17916594]
5. Viggedal G,Lundalv E,Carlsson G,Kjellmer I. Follow-up into young adulthood after cardiopulmonary resuscitation in term and near-term newborn infants. II. Neuropsychological consequencesActa Paediatr 2002;91:1218–1226. [pmid: 12463322]
6. Blackman JA. The value of Apgar scores in predicting developmental outcome at age fiveJ Perinatol 1988;8:206–210. [pmid: 3225662]
7. O'Donnell CP,Kamlin CO,Davis PG,Carlin JB,Morley CJ. Interobserver variability of the 5-minute Apgar scoreJ Pediatr 2006;149:486–489. [pmid: 17011319]
8. Myers RE. Two patterns of perinatal brain damage and their conditions of occurrenceAm J Obstet Gynecol 1972;112:246–276. [pmid: 4621486]
9. Baskett TF,Allen VM,O'Connell CM,Allen AC. Predictors of respiratory depression at birth in the term infantBJOG 2006;113:769–774. [pmid: 16827759]
10. Villar J,de Onis M,Kestler E,Bolanos F,Cerezo R,Bernedes H. The differential neonatal morbidity of the intrauterine growth retardation syndromeAm J Obstet Gynecol 1990;163:151–157. [pmid: 2375339]
11. Badawi N,Kurinczuk JJ,Keogh JM. Antepartum risk factors for newborn encephalopathy: the Western Australian case-control studyBMJ 1998;317:1549–1553. [pmid: 9836652]
12. Badawi N,Kurinczuk JJ,Keogh JM. Intrapartum risk factors for newborn encephalopathy: the Western Australian case-control studyBMJ 1998;317:1554–1558. [pmid: 9836653]
13. Sarnat HB,Sarnat MS. Neonatal encephalopathy following fetal distress. A clinical and electroencephalographic studyArch Neurol 1976;33:696–705. [pmid: 987769]
14. Wechsler D. Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (3rd edn.). 3rd edn.. 1991Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX:
15. Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-IV UK) (4th edn.). 4th edn.. 2004Harcourt Assessment; London:
16. Office of Population Censuses and SurveysStandard Occupational Classification vols 1, 2, and 31991;HM Stationary Office; London:
17. Greenland S,Drescher K. Maximum likelihood estimation of the attributable fraction from logistic modelsBiometrics 1993;49:865–872. [pmid: 8241375]
18. Royston P. Multiple imputation of missing values: updateStata J 2005;5:681–2001.
19. Kaufman A. Intelligent testing with the WISC-III. 1994John Wiley & Sons; New York, NY:
20. Veltman DJ,de Ruiter MB,Rombouts SA. Neurophysiological correlates of increased verbal working memory in high-dissociative participants: a functional MRI studyPsychol Med 2005;35:175–185. [pmid: 15841675]
21. Pasquier F,Lebert F,Grymonprez L,Petit H. Verbal fluency in dementia of frontal lobe type and dementia of Alzheimer typeJ Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1995;58:81–84. [pmid: 7823074]
22. Dutton GN,Jacobson LK. Cerebral visual impairment in childrenSemin Neonatol 2001;6:477–485. [pmid: 12014888]
23. Peters TJ,Golding J,Lawrence CJ,Fryer JG,Chamberlain GV,Butler NR. Delayed onset of regular respiration and subsequent developmentEarly Hum Dev 1984;9:225–239. [pmid: 6734485]
24. Golding J,Pembrey M,Jones R,the ALSPAC Study TeamALSPAC—the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. I. Study methodologyPaediatr Perinat Epidemiol 2001;15:74–87. [pmid: 11237119]
25. Palme-Kilander C. Methods of resuscitation in low-Apgar-score newborn infants—a national surveyActa Paediatr 1992;81:739–744. [pmid: 1421875]
26. Kroll L,Twohey L,Daubeney PE,Lynch D,Ducker DA. Risk factors at delivery and the need for skilled resuscitationEur J Obstet Gynecol Reprod Biol 1994;55:175–177. [pmid: 7958161]
27. Resuscitation Council (UK)Resuscitation at birth: the newborn life support provider course manual. 2001Resuscitation Council (UK); London:
28. Tan A,Schulze A,O'Donnell CP,Davis PG. Air versus oxygen for resuscitation of infants at birthCochrane Database Syst Rev 2005;2 CD002273.
29. Saugstad OD. Take a breath—but do not add oxygen (if not needed)Acta Paediatr 2007;96:798–800. [pmid: 17537006]
30. Draycott T,Sibanda T,Owen L. Does training in obstetric emergencies improve neonatal outcome?BJOG 2006;113:177–182. [pmid: 16411995]

Article Categories:
  • Articles


Previous Document:  A short cytochemical note on the nucleolar and cytoplasmic RNA concentration in differentiating cell...
Next Document:  CDKN1C mutations in HELLP/preeclamptic mothers of Beckwith-Wiedemann Syndrome (BWS) patients.