Document Detail

Meta review of systematic and meta analytic reviews on movement differences, effect of movement based interventions, and the underlying neural mechanisms in autism spectrum disorder.
Jump to Full Text
MedLine Citation:
PMID:  23532374     Owner:  NLM     Status:  PubMed-not-MEDLINE    
Abstract/OtherAbstract:
PURPOSES: To identify and appraise evidence from published systematic and meta analytic reviews on (1) movement differences of individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASD); (2) the effects of movement based interventions for ASD; (3) hypothesized underlying neural mechanisms for the movement characteristics.
METHODS: A meta review of published systematic and meta analytic reviews on movement differences, structural, and functional brain anomalies in ASD and the effects of movement based interventions for individuals with ASD between 1806 and October 2012. The methodological quality of the identified systematic and meta analytic reviews was independently assessed by two assessors with the assessment of multiple systematic reviews (AMSTAR).
RESULTS: The search yielded a total of 12 reviews that met the inclusion/exclusion criteria. The methodological quality of the reviews varied, but the review conclusions were similar. Although individuals with ASD generally perform less well than age-matched controls in developmental movement tasks, there are few exceptions whose movement abilities are intact. Most movement based interventions report their efficacies. However, all existing studies employ the research design that is inherently incapable of providing strong evidence, and they often fail to report the extent of psychosocial interactions within the movement interventions. The hypothesized neural mechanisms are still under development and speculative in nature.
CONCLUSIONS: It is premature to designate movement disturbance as a core symptom of ASD. The effects of movement based interventions on the present ASD core symptoms need to be further validated by stronger evidence and verified theoretical mechanisms linking ASD with movement disorders.
Authors:
Motohide Miyahara
Related Documents :
11295814 - Atypical fibroxanthoma of the scalp.
6858664 - Indication for weight relief and containment in the treatment of perthes' disease.
7794504 - A metabolic complication of severe burns.
22222434 - Erosive bladder perforation as a complication of ventriculoperitoneal shunt with extrus...
1519964 - Necrobacillosis with pancytopenia.
22019654 - Multifocal motor neuropathy.
Publication Detail:
Type:  Journal Article     Date:  2013-03-26
Journal Detail:
Title:  Frontiers in integrative neuroscience     Volume:  7     ISSN:  1662-5145     ISO Abbreviation:  Front Integr Neurosci     Publication Date:  2013  
Date Detail:
Created Date:  2013-03-27     Completed Date:  2013-03-28     Revised Date:  2014-07-31    
Medline Journal Info:
Nlm Unique ID:  101477950     Medline TA:  Front Integr Neurosci     Country:  Switzerland    
Other Details:
Languages:  eng     Pagination:  16     Citation Subset:  -    
Export Citation:
APA/MLA Format     Download EndNote     Download BibTex
MeSH Terms
Descriptor/Qualifier:

From MEDLINE®/PubMed®, a database of the U.S. National Library of Medicine

Full Text
Journal Information
Journal ID (nlm-ta): Front Integr Neurosci
Journal ID (iso-abbrev): Front Integr Neurosci
Journal ID (publisher-id): Front. Integr. Neurosci.
ISSN: 1662-5145
Publisher: Frontiers Media S.A.
Article Information
Download PDF
Copyright © 2013 Miyahara.
open-access:
Received Day: 29 Month: 12 Year: 2012
Accepted Day: 06 Month: 3 Year: 2013
Electronic publication date: Day: 26 Month: 3 Year: 2013
collection publication date: Year: 2013
Volume: 7E-location ID: 16
PubMed Id: 23532374
ID: 3607787
DOI: 10.3389/fnint.2013.00016

Meta review of systematic and meta analytic reviews on movement differences, effect of movement based interventions, and the underlying neural mechanisms in autism spectrum disorder
Motohide Miyahara*
Movement Development Clinic, School of Physical Education, University of OtagoDunedin, New Zealand
[edited-by] Edited by: Anne M. Donnellan, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Uzbekistan
[edited-by] Reviewed by: Nandakumar Narayanan, Yale University, USA; Elizabeth B. Torres, Rutgers University, USA
Correspondence: *Correspondence: Motohide Miyahara, Movement Development Clinic, School of Physical Education, University of Otago, 46 Union Street, PO Box 56, Dunedin 9054, New Zealand. e-mail: motohide.miyahara@otago.ac.nz

Introduction

The core symptoms of autism spectrum disorders (ASD) encompass impairments in social interaction and communication, as well as circumscribed interest and fixed behaviors (American Psychiatric Association, 2012). A cross-cutting dimension of these clinical signs is the development of movement skills that are observable, functional, goal-oriented, and acquired as a result of practice. For example, the movements of the vocal cord and the other body parts are essential for communicative speech, gesture, writing, and typing. Stereotypies and preoccupation can be only inferred from observed physical movement patterns. Thus, movement disturbances seem to co-exist with ASD, but the relations between the two disorders have not been critically appraised or adequately synthesized from the previous reviews.

In this meta review, I will evaluate the methodological quality of reviews, re-examine the strength of evidence, the homogeneity of studies and research designs, and integrate the findings to answer my three research questions: movement differences, effect of movement based interventions, and the underlying neuronal mechanisms that link ASD with movement disturbance.


Methods

My search was based on the electronic databases available at the University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand: Web of Knowledge (1898–2012), Medline (1950–2012), PsychINFO (1806–2012), EMBASE (1980–2012), and ERIC (1987–2012) on June 12, 2012. I used the combinations of three or four keywords from the following inclusion criteria: (1) type of paper: systematic review or meta analysis; (2) population or comparison: autism, pervasive developmental disorders, autism spectrum disorder, Asperger disorder; (3) intervention: sport, exercise, physical exercise, physical activity; (4) primary outcome: movement skills, motor skills, motor coordination, physical fitness, imitation; secondary outcome: social skills, autistic behaviors, self-perception, and academics; (5) magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). I included only published studies in English, and excluded “gray” and “figitive” literature. To conduct duplicate study selection, a reference librarian served as a reviews search coordinator. Any inconsistencies in our results were resolved through subsequent collaborative search, reading of respective journal articles, and discussion.

The content of identified review studies was assessed with respect to the professional field of reviewers, type and topic of reviews, numbers of reviewed studies, year range of the studies, total number of subjects, level of evidence, review method, outcome variables, and theoretical interpretation. The methodological quality of the reviews was assessed by two independent assessors with the assessment of multiple systematic review (AMSTAR) tool (Shea et al., 2007). Disagreements were resolved by discussion.


Results

Our search yielded a total of 12 studies. As shown in Table 1, four studies examined the existing evidence of movement differences, the other four evaluated the evidence of movement based treatment effects, and the remaining four studies synthesized the foregoing studies that investigated the neuronal mechanisms that link ASD and movement disturbance. I excluded three studies (Redcay and Courchesne, 2005; Sugranyes et al., 2011; Ipser et al., 2012) from further analysis because they did not address my research questions directly. The AMSTAR methodological quality scores of the 12 reviewed studies ranged from 20 (Emck et al., 2009) to 67% (Petrus et al., 2008; Stanfield et al., 2008) of the relevant criteria (Table 2).


Movement differences of ASD

Of the four objective reviews available on this topic, Fournier et al.'s study (2010) is the only standard meta analytic review on movement differences between individuals with ASD and controls. The remaining three systematic reviews consist of Emck et al.'s (2009) examination of gross motor performance and self-perceived motor competence in ASD, Downey and Rapport's (2012) examination of the movement differences that limit motor activity, and Williams et al.'s (2004) investigation into the differential imitation ability in which they attempted to quantify the movement differences based on p-values. Thus, the four reviews analyzed different yet partially overlapping aspects of movements with each distinctive method.

Fournier et al. (2010) provide the only standard meta analysis that examines motor difference in ASD. As evident on the relatively high AMSTAR score (Table 2), their methodological procedures are fairly meticulous. They found a large effect (ES = 1.20) which they called motor coordination deficit in ASD. Upon scrutiny, however, the variables included in the meta analysis were not only standard assessment of motor coordination, but also sensory motor measures, the measures of letter height (Beversdorf et al., 2001), neurological soft signs (Tani et al., 2006), and even an indicator of the cognitive executive function (Coldren and Halloran, 2003).

Inheritant to meta analysis is the “apples and oranges” problem in that data of different nature are analyzed together, and this criticism may be overruled if the analysis aims to generalize to a higher-order class as “fruit” (Matt and Cook, 2009). Though Fournier et al. name the higher-order class of meta analysed dependent variables “motor coordination,” specific sensory motor measures are not usually considered as the indicators of motor coordination in assessing developmental coordination disorder (DCD). Because the proposed DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2012) allows dual diagnoses of ASD and DCD, it is more meaningful and clinically practical to exclusively use the standard assessment of motor coordination. Fournier et al.'s moderator variable analyses were limited to subtypes of ASD, without including the types of movement measures as a moderator variable. Hence, I computed a random effects model meta analysis on the six comparisons in the five studies that had used standard motor coordination measures. The aggregated standardized mean difference effect was significant, 2.91 (SE = 0.581; p < 0.001; Z = 5.01; I2 = 93.48; 95% CI = 1.774−4.051), larger than 1.20, the effect size of the all 51 studies, suggesting a high degree of comorbidity of ASD with DCD.

Emck et al.'s (2009) systematic review only covered gross motor performance and perceived competence. The authors concluded that children with ASD were clearly impaired in gross motor development. It is noteworthy that they acknowledge the existence of children with ASD whose gross motor performance fell within the normal range. Because this review neither evaluated the strength of evidence nor attempted quantitative synthesis, their conclusions need to be interpreted with caution.

Downey and Rapport (2012) categorized the movement differences into early motor development, gesture and motor imitation, postural control, and dyspraxia. The strategy for their literature search was described clearly, but the strength of evidence was not evaluated in this systematic review. The movement differences were summed up as activity limitations (World Health Organization, 2001) which should take both biological and social aspects into account. However, the authors focused on individual functional adaptation, and recommended physical therapists to promote functional intervention without adequately addressing the needs for social and environmental accommodation. It is open to question whether all aspects of movement differences actually reflect the limitations in motor activity.

Williams et al. (2004) conducted a review on the difference in action imitation between individuals with ASD and matched controls. Although the levels of evidence were not specifically evaluated, the authors ensured the quality of reviewed studies by only including those studies that had employed control groups into their review. Out of thus selected 21 studies, 17 studies were pooled into a meta analysis. The combined logit p-value <0.0005 indicated a significant group difference.

In summary, a meta review of the four review studies indicates substantial movement differences between young people with ASD and their typically developing counterparts through a limited quality and quantity of data synthesis. However, not all individuals with ASD have DCD (Emck et al., 2009). Therefore, movement disturbance cannot constitute a core symptom of ASD. If present, a comorbid diagnosis of DCD should be given in accordance with the proposed DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2012).


Effects of movement based interventions

Four studies (Baranek, 2002; Petrus et al., 2008; Lang et al., 2010; Sowa and Meulenbroek, 2012) examined the effects of movement based interventions in individuals with ASD. Only Sowa and Meulenbroek (2012) conducted a meta analysis, and the other studies reviewed foregoing studies systematically. All four studies included the effects of physical exercise on ASD symptoms and movement functions.

In addition to the effects of physical exercise, which will be elaborated later with more recent reviews, Baranek (2002) covered sensory- and motor-based interventions, such as sensory integration therapy and sensory stimulation techniques, which have decreased in popularity recently. Although slightly outdated, her review is comprehensive for the covered range of movement based interventions. She also pointed out the issue of internal validity, particularly the degree of psychosocial interaction that occurred during physical exercise. This information is crucial to determine whether physical exercise alone or a combination of exercise and interpersonal interaction altered dependent variables. She maintained that the effect of physical exercise would be specific to the context of physical exercise, and it would not be generalized or transferred to social play.

Petrus et al.'s (2008) systematic review on exercise intervention effects targeted the outcome variable of stereotypic behaviors in children with ASD. They reviewed seven studies which used either the case series or case study design with small sample sizes (N < 6). Petrus et al. classified the studies by Kern et al. (1982) and Kern et al. (1984) as Level II: smaller RCT. However, these studies had no control group, and therefore, should be reclassified as Level IV: case series. Given the weak evidence of the reviewed studies, Petrus et al.'s claim of “weak to moderately strong evidence” (p. 142) should be revised to weak evidence.

Lang et al.'s (2010) systematic review overlaps with Petrus et al.'s (2008) review, but included wider outcome variables other than stereotypic behaviors. As in the case of Petrus et al.'s (2008) review, the appraised studies were characterized by small sample sizes (N < 9) and the use of time-series analysis to evaluate the intervention outcome (7 of the 18 studies). This review provided critical information that 15 of the 18 studies had involved teaching exercise, mostly jogging, to individuals with ASD by modeling, physical guidance, verbal reinforcement and contingency management. One study even used jogging in social plays, such as follow-the-leader and tag. Such a psychosocial component in the “physical” intervention could explain the improvement in the behavioral and academic domains, as well as in the physical domain. The authors narratively evaluated the research methodologies and the intervention outcomes, but neither categorical classification of evidence levels nor meta analysis was performed. Yet Lang et al. (2010) acknowledged the limitations of reviewed research being the fact that no research employed the experimental design, but depended on time-series analysis. An advantage of time-series analysis lies in its capability to infer the effect of intervention on an individual without considering inter-individual differences. The inevitable corollary of the advantage reduces generalizability. Hence, the reported benefits of physical exercises from the time-series data need to be confirmed by randomized control trials.

Sowa and Meulenbroek (2012) searched movement based intervention studies published between 1991 and 2011 with respect to the effects of the interventions on the physical and psychosocial domains in people with ASD. Of the 16 studies they identified, seven studies conducted individual physical exercise programs and nine studies administered group programs. All activity programs yielded significant progress on the assessed measures, but the individual programmes elicited significantly more improvement than the group interventions not only in the movement domain, but also in the social domain. A question may be raised as to whether physical exercise indirectly triggered the improvement in social functions through a yet unknown mechanism, or the psychosocial interactions that occurred during the physical exercise programs directly enhanced the social skills. Unfortunately, a majority of the original studies cited in the review failed to report the extent of psychosocial interactions in the “physical” exercises, making it difficult to determine the cause of the improvement in the social domain.

As the analytic method of the improvement rates indicates, Sowa and Meulenbroek's analysis examines only the differences before and after the interventions without considering the control groups. Indeed, this is still a meta analysis in the sense of aggregating studies, such an analytic method falls in Grade III-3 to IV level of evidence, which is regarded as either satisfactory or poor (NHMRC, 2000). The absence of the control groups in the analysis does not allow us to tease out the intervention effects from confounding factors, such as Hawthorne effect. No matter how large the combined sample size may be, no definite conclusion can be drawn from Sowa and Meulenbroek's meta analysis with regard to the causal effects of the individual or group motor interventions on the motor or the psychosocial domain. On an additional note, the authors seem to be unfamiliar with the difficulties that individuals with ASD face while engaging in complex team sports in which the environment changes constantly, as the authors wonder why there is no “naturalistic group-based sport intervention like soccer” (p. 56).

Thus, all four reviews reported the benefit of movement based interventions, physical exercise, in particular, on the physical and psychosocial domains. On closer scrutiny however, the extent of psychosocial intervention within the movement based interventions has not been quantified or partialed out, but confounded. Coupled with the limited generalizability of time-series analysis employed by a majority of the reviewed studies, the reported effects need to be interpreted cautiously with understanding of these limitations.


Theoretical models linking movement disorders and ASD

Six out of the eight reviews on motor differences or motor interventions offered theoretical insights into the relations between ASD and motor functions, ranging from causal directions between the motor and the social domains to underlying neural structures and functions (Table 1).

In their systematic review, Downey and Rapport (2012) raised a question whether limited social behaviors of individuals with ASD prevented them from learning motor skills, or poor motor functions impoverished social life. This is a curious question, but it is extremely difficult to establish clear-cut causalities for methodological reasons.

Four review studies speculated the underlying mechanism between movement and autistic disorders. On the functional level, Williams et al. (2004) attributed the movement difference to an ASD specific deficit in self-other mapping ability particularly for the imitation tasks that have low congruence between semantic and visuomotor couplings. On the physiological level, Baranek (2002) cited Kern et al. (1982) and Kern et al. (1984) to explain the benefit of physical exercise in term of physiological responses, such as the secretion of neurotransmitters, beta-endorphins, and acetylcholine. Emck et al. (2009) attributed the co-occurrence of the multiple impairments to “an abnormal connectivity of brain system” (p. 512) based on functional correlations between motor, cognitive, and socio-emotional impairments. More specifically, Fournier et al. (2010) attribute motor dysfunctions to abnormalities in fronto-striatal connections and basal ganglia, based on the foregoing neurophysiological studies. Four reviews on actual neurophysiological studies provide us with the “state of art” hindsight of structural and functional MRI studies.

Stanfield et al. (2008) meta analyzed 46 volumetric MRI studies on regional brain size in ASD by adjusting age and IQ. The cerebrum and the cerebellum (vermal lobules VI-VII and VIII-X) of the ASD were larger than the control, whereas the corpus callosum was smaller in size. The authors related the enlarged cerebellum and its presumably disorganized connection with the cerebral regions to motor dysfunction, as well as cognitive and socio-emotional dysfunctions in ASD. Thus, the authors ascribed the link between movement disturbance and ASD to morphological changes in the brain.

In their systematic review and meta analysis of functional MRI research on ASD, Philip et al. (2012) identified three studies which had employed motor tasks of button pressing in MRI scanners. Compared to controls, individuals with ASD showed significantly different activation patterns (either hyper- or hypo-activation) in the motor regions (e.g., the cerebellum, the precentral gyrus, the basal ganglia) and attentional systems (the basal ganglia, the superior and inferior parietal lobules). The authors related the ASD groups' hyper-activation in the right inferior frontal gyrus and the hypo-activation in the left inferior parietal lobule to the mirror neuron system hypothesis in that these regions were involved in observation and execution of model movements. Note that Philip et al. were conservative in that they made no link between the differential brain activation patterns and the movement difference in ASD or the effect of movement based intervention on ASD.

Müller et al. (2011) reviewed 32 functional connectivity MRI studies of ASD, and found 22 studies supported the general underconnectivity hypothesis, whereas 11 studies did not support the hypothesis. The authors recognized the diversity in data analysis, suggesting that the discrepant findings might depend on each study's methodology. They interpreted underconnectivity as decreased efficiency of network interactions, and the increased functional connectivity as a malformation in experience-driven network. Müller et al. (2011) believed that all results represented the anomaly in white matter development which resulted in ASD symptomatology encompassing social, communicative, and movement disorders.

Nickl-Jockschat et al. (2012) meta analyzed 16 morphometric MRI studies and linked disturbances in the left pericental region, the left putamen, the right caudate, and the right parietal operculum with sensorymotor impairment in ASD.

The theoretical links between movement disorder and ASD have been explored in terms of the directions of influence and the neurophysiological levels. It is difficult to establish the direction of causalities. None of the neurophysiological evidence sufficiently accounts for the movement differences in ASD or why movement based interventions result in the improvement in the motor and psychosocial domains in ASD.


Conclusive remarks

Accumulated studies indicate significant movement differences between ASD and control groups. However, the existence of individuals with ASD, who are free from movement problems, does not warrant the designation of movement disturbance as a core symptom of ASD. There is moderate to low quality evidence for the effects of movement based intervention on the motor, behavior, and psychosocial domains. Coupled with the limited descriptions on the psychosocial interactions during the movement based interventions, future research on the process and the outcome of movement based intervention needs to control and examine the interactions more precisely. Neurophysiological accounts for the movement differences and the effects of movement based intervention range from the size of brain regions, differential brain activation patterns during motor tasks, and functional connectivity. While none of these theoretical hypotheses can directly explain the movement differences or the effects of movement based interventions, they serve as useful theoretical models to be refined and tested in further research.

Conflict of interest statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.


I would like to thank Justin Farquhar for severing as a reviews search coordinator, and Losia Lagisz for independently assessing the selected review studies with the AMSTAR.


References
American Psychiatric Association. (Year: 2012). DSM-5 Development. [Online]. American Psychiatric Association. Available online at: http://www.dsm5.org (Accessed October 29, 2012).
Baranek G. T.. (Year: 2002). Efficacy of sensory and motor interventions for children with autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 32, 397–42212463517
Beversdorf D. Q.,Anderson J. M.,Manning S. E.,Anderson S. L.,Nordgren R. E.,Felopulos G. J.,et al. (Year: 2001). Brief report: macrographia in high-functioning adults with autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 31, 97–10111439759
Coldren J. T.,Halloran C.. (Year: 2003). Spatial reversal as a measure of executive functioning in children with autism. J. Genet. Psychol. 164, 29–4110.1080/0022132030959750112693742
Downey R.,Rapport M. J. K.. (Year: 2012). Motor activity in children with autism: a review of current literature. Pediatr. Phys. Ther. 24, 2–2010.1097/PEP.0b013e31823db95f22207460
Emck C.,Bosscher R.,Beek P.,Doreleijers T.. (Year: 2009). Gross motor performance and self-perceived motor competence in children with emotional, behavioural, and pervasive developmental disorders: a review. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 51, 501–51710.1111/j.1469-8749.2009.03337.x19538424
Fournier K. A.,Hass C. J.,Naik S. K.,Lodha N.,Cauraugh J. H.. (Year: 2010). Motor coordination in autism spectrum disorders: a synthesis and meta-analysis. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 40, 1227–124010.1007/s10803-010-0981-320195737
Ipser J. C.,Syal S.,Bentley J.,Adnams C. M.,Steyn B.,Stein D. J.. (Year: 2012). 1H-MRS in autism spectrum disorders: a systematic meta-analysis. Metab. Brain Dis. 27, 275–28710.1007/s11011-012-9293-y22426803
Kern L.,Koegel R. L.,Dunlap G.. (Year: 1984). The influence of vigorous versus mild exercise on autistic stereotyped behaviors. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 14, 57–676706897
Kern L.,Koegel R. L.,Dyer K.,Blew P. A.,Fenton L. R.. (Year: 1982). The effects of physical exercise on self-stimulation and appropriate responding in autistic-children. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 12, 399–4197161239
Lang R.,Koegel L. K.,Ashbaugh K.,Regester A.,Ence W.,Smith W.. (Year: 2010). Physical exercise and individuals with autism spectrum disorders: a systematic review. Res. Autism Spectr. Dis. 4, 565–57610.1002/mrdd.2001715362162
Matt G. E.,Cook T. D.. (Year: 2009). Threats to the validity of generalized inferences, in The Handbook of Research Synthesis and Meta-Analysis, 2nd Edn., eds Cooper H.,Hedges L. V.,Valentine J. C. (New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation), 537–560
Müller R. A.,Shih P.,Keehn B.,Deyoe J. R.,Leyden K. M.,Shukla D. K.. (Year: 2011). Underconnected, but how? A survey of functional connectivity MRI studies in autism spectrum disorders. Cereb. Cortex21, 2233–224310.1093/cercor/bhq29621378114
NHMRC. (Year: 2000). How to Use the Evidence: Assessment and Application of Scientific Evidence. Canberra, ACT: National Health and Medical Research Council
Nickl-Jockschat T.,Habel U.,Michel T. M.,Manning J.,Laird A. R.,Fox P. T.,et al. (Year: 2012). Brain structure anomalies in autism spectrum disorder—a meta-analysis of VBM studies using anatomic likelihood estimation. Hum. Brain Mapp. 33, 1470–148910.1002/hbm.2129921692142
Petrus C.,Adamson S. R.,Block L.,Einarson S. J.,Sharifnejad M.,Harris S. R.. (Year: 2008). Effects of exercise interventions on stereotypic behaviours in children with autism spectrum disorder. Physiother. Can. 60, 134–14510.3138/physio.60.2.13420145777
Philip R. C. M.,Dauvermann M. R.,Whalley H. C.,Baynham K.,Lawrie S. M.,Stanfield A. C.. (Year: 2012). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the fMRI investigation of autism spectrum disorders. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 901–94210.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.10.00822101112
Redcay E.,Courchesne E.. (Year: 2005). When is the brain enlarged in autism? A meta-analysis of all brain size reports. Biol. Psychiatry58, 1–910.1016/j.biopsych.2005.03.02615935993
Shea B. J.,Grimshaw J. M.,Wells G. A.,Boers M.,Andersson N.,Hamel C.,et al. (Year: 2007). Development of AMSTAR: a measurement tool to assess the methodological quality of systematic reviews. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 7:1010.1186/1471-2288-7-1017302989
Sowa M.,Meulenbroek R.. (Year: 2012). Effects of physical exercise on autism spectrum disorders: a meta-analysis. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 6, 46–57
Stanfield A. C.,McIntosh A. M.,Spencer M. D.,Philip R.,Gaur S.,Lawrie S. M.. (Year: 2008). Towards a neuroanatomy of autism: a systematic review and meta-analysis of structural magnetic resonance imaging studies. Eur. Psychiatry23, 289–29910.1016/j.eurpsy.2007.05.00617765485
Sugranyes G.,Kyriakopoulos M.,Corrigall R.,Taylor E.,Frangou S.. (Year: 2011). Autism spectrum disorders and schizophrenia: meta-analysis of the neural correlates of social cognition. PLoS ONE6:e2532210.1371/journal.pone.002532221998649
Tani P.,Lindberg N.,Appelberg B.,Nieminen-von Wendt T.,von Wendt L.,Porkka-Heiskanen T.. (Year: 2006). Clinical neurological abnormalities in young adults with Asperger syndrome. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 60, 253–25510.1111/j.1440-1819.2006.01494.x16594952
Williams J. H. G.,Whiten A.,Singh T.. (Year: 2004). A systematic review of action imitation in autistic spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 34, 285–29915264497
World Health Organization. (Year: 2001). International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health (ICF). Geneva: World Health Organization

Tables
[TableWrap ID: T1] Table 1 

Characteristics of reviews which met the inclusion criteria (N = 12).


First author (Year) Professional field of the reviewers Type of review Topic of review No. of reviewed studies Year range of the studies Total no. of subjects Levels of reviewed studies Method used in reviewed studies Outcome variables Theoretical interpretations offered
Williams (2004) Pediatrics, Psychology Systematic Imitation difference 21 1966–2002 281 Not assessed Comparative Action imitation Yes
Emck (2009) Kinesiology, Psychiatry Systematic Movement difference 5* 1997–2007 Not reported Not assessed Comparative Gross motor performance, self-perception Yes
Fournier (2010) Kinesiology Meta analytic Movement difference 51 1980–2009 Not reported Not assessed Comparative Motor coordination Yes
Downey (2012) Physicla Therapy Systematic Movement difference 49 Not reported Not reported Level 2–3 Comparative Motor development, imitation, posture, dyspraxia Yes
Baranek (2002) Education Systematic Treatment effect 4 1980–1993 Not reported Level 3–4 Case series Sensory motor functions, behavior Yes
Petrus (2008) Physicla Therapy Systematic Treatment effect 7 1982–2003 25 Level 2–5 Case series Stereotypic behaviors No
Lang (2010) Education Systematic Treatment effect 18 1974–2007 64 Not assessed Case series Behavior, academics, physical fitness Yes
Sowa (2012) Neuroscience Meta analytic Treatment effect 16 1991–2011 133 Not assessed Case series Motor skills, social skills No
Stanfield (2008) Psychiatry Meta analytic Structural difference 46 1984–2006 Over 1600 Not assessed Comparative Brain volume Yes
Müller (2011) Psychology, Neuroscience Systematic Connectivity difference 32 2004–2010 Not reported Not assessed Comparative Functional connectivity Yes
Philip (2012) Psychiatry Meta analytic Brain activation difference 3 1984–2009 24 Not assessed Comparative Brain activation Yes
Nickl-Jockschat (2012) Psychiatry, Neuroscience Meta analytic Structural difference 16 1999–2009 580 Not assessed Comparative Structural changes Yes

Note: All Meta analytic reviews included systematic reviews.

*No. of movement difference studies only;

No. of movement intervention studies only;

No. of movement task studies only.


[TableWrap ID: T2] Table 2 

Methodological quality evaluated by the assessment of multiple systematic reviews tool (AMSTAR) (Shea et al., 2007).


First author (Year) 1. Was an “a priori” design provided? 2. Was there duplicate study selection and data extraction? 3. Was a comprehensive literature search performed? 4. Was the status of publication (i.e., gray literature) used as an inclusion criterion? 5. Was a list of studies (included and excluded) provided? 6. Were characteristics of included studies provided? 7. Was the scientific quality of the included studies assessed and documented? 8. Was the scientific quality of the included studies used appropriately in formulating conclusions? 9. Were the methods used to combine the findings of studies appropriate? 10. Was the likelihood of publication bias assessed? 11. Were potential conflicts of interest included? Total score Percent
Williams(2004) CA CA Yes CA No Yes Yes Yes NA NA No 4/9 44
Emck (2009) CA CA CA CA No Yes Yes No NA No No 2/10 20
Fournier (2010) CA Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes No No Yes No 7/11 64
Downey (2012) CA CA No No No Yes Yes No NA NA No 2/9 22
Baranek (2002) CA No Yes CA No Yes Yes Yes NA NA No 4/9 44
Petrus (2008) CA Yes Yes Yes No Yes Yes Yes NA NA No 6/9 67
Lang (2010) CA Yes Yes No No Yes Yes Yes NA NA No 5/9 56
Sowa (2012) CA CA No CA No Yes No No No Yes No 2/11 18
Stanfield (2008) CA Yes Yes Yes No Yes NA NA Yes Yes No 6/9 67
Müller (2011) CA Yes No No No Yes NA NA Yes No No 3/9 33
Philip (2012) CA CA Yes Yes No Yes NA NA Yes No No 4/9 44
Nickl-Jockschat (2012) CA CA CA No No Yes NA NA Yes No No 2/9 22

Note: CA, can't answer; NA, not applicable.



Article Categories:
  • Neuroscience
    • Mini Review Article

Keywords: autism, movement, developmental coordination disorder, motor development, MRI and fMRI.

Previous Document:  Abrogation of IL-4 receptor-?-dependent alternatively activated macrophages is sufficient to confer ...
Next Document:  ERP evidence suggests executive dysfunction in ecstasy polydrug users.