Document Detail

Incestuous sisters: mate preference for brothers over unrelated males in Drosophila melanogaster.
Jump to Full Text
MedLine Citation:
PMID:  23251487     Owner:  NLM     Status:  MEDLINE    
Abstract/OtherAbstract:
The literature is full of examples of inbreeding avoidance, while recent mathematical models predict that inbreeding tolerance or even inbreeding preference should be expected under several realistic conditions like e.g. polygyny. We investigated male and female mate preferences with respect to relatedness in the fruit fly D. melanogaster. Experiments offered the choice between a first order relative (full-sibling or parent) and an unrelated individual with the same age and mating history. We found that females significantly preferred mating with their brothers, thus supporting inbreeding preference. Moreover, females did not avoid mating with their fathers, and males did not avoid mating with their sisters, thus supporting inbreeding tolerance. Our experiments therefore add empirical evidence for inbreeding preference, which strengthens the prediction that inbreeding tolerance and preference can evolve under specific circumstances through the positive effects on inclusive fitness.
Authors:
Adeline Loyau; Jérémie H Cornuau; Jean Clobert; Etienne Danchin
Related Documents :
23098897 - Small and poor females change sex: a theoretical and empirical study on protogynous sex...
20437767 - Quantitative pcr of propionibacterium acnes dna in samples aspirated from sebaceous fol...
12717857 - Contents of chemical elements in stomach during prenatal development: different age-dep...
23505417 - Prediction of age at menopause from assessment of ovarian reserve may be improved by us...
22037537 - Universal prevention is associated with lower prevalence of fetal alcohol spectrum diso...
18071037 - Variability of age at onset in siblings with familial alzheimer disease.
Publication Detail:
Type:  Journal Article; Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't     Date:  2012-12-10
Journal Detail:
Title:  PloS one     Volume:  7     ISSN:  1932-6203     ISO Abbreviation:  PLoS ONE     Publication Date:  2012  
Date Detail:
Created Date:  2012-12-19     Completed Date:  2013-06-11     Revised Date:  2013-07-11    
Medline Journal Info:
Nlm Unique ID:  101285081     Medline TA:  PLoS One     Country:  United States    
Other Details:
Languages:  eng     Pagination:  e51293     Citation Subset:  IM    
Affiliation:
CNRS, Station d'Ecologie Expérimentale du CNRS à Moulis, USR 2936, Saint Girons, France. al@adeline-loyau.net
Export Citation:
APA/MLA Format     Download EndNote     Download BibTex
MeSH Terms
Descriptor/Qualifier:
Animals
Drosophila melanogaster / physiology*
Female
Male
Sexual Behavior, Animal*
Comments/Corrections

From MEDLINE®/PubMed®, a database of the U.S. National Library of Medicine

Full Text
Journal Information
Journal ID (nlm-ta): PLoS One
Journal ID (iso-abbrev): PLoS ONE
Journal ID (publisher-id): plos
Journal ID (pmc): plosone
ISSN: 1932-6203
Publisher: Public Library of Science, San Francisco, USA
Article Information
Download PDF
Copyright: 2012 Loyau et al
License:
Received Day: 3 Month: 7 Year: 2012
Accepted Day: 1 Month: 11 Year: 2012
collection publication date: Year: 2012
Electronic publication date: Day: 10 Month: 12 Year: 2012
Volume: 7 Issue: 12
E-location ID: e51293
PubMed Id: 23251487
ID: 3519633
Publisher Id: PONE-D-12-20625
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0051293

Incestuous Sisters: Mate Preference for Brothers over Unrelated Males in Drosophila melanogaster Alternate Title:Inbreeding Preference in the Fruit Fly
Adeline Loyau1234*
Jérémie H. Cornuau1
Jean Clobert1
Étienne Danchin23
Trine Bildeedit1 Role: Editor
1CNRS, Station d’Ecologie Expérimentale du CNRS à Moulis, USR 2936, Saint Girons, France
2CNRS, Université Paul Sabatier, ENFA, Laboratoire d'Evolution et Diversité Biologique (EDB), UMR 5174, Toulouse, France
3Université de Toulouse, UMR 5174, Toulouse, France
4Department of Conservation Biology, Helmholtz Centre for Environmental Research – UFZ, Leipzig, Germany
Aarhus University, Denmark
Correspondence: * E-mail: al@adeline-loyau.net
[conflict] Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Contributed by footnote: Conceived and designed the experiments: AL JHC. Performed the experiments: AL. Analyzed the data: AL. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: ÉD JC. Wrote the paper: AL. Contributed to materials and salary: ÉD JC. Revised the paper critically for important intellectual content: JHC JC ÉD.

Introduction

Until recently, it has been the norm to assume that inbreeding avoidance should be the rule of thumb in mate choice under most circumstances because mating with relatives often causes a reduction in the fitness of inbred offspring. This reduction of fitness, which is called inbreeding depression, is due to either a loss of heterozygosity or an increased expression of recessive deleterious alleles [1]. The fitness costs of inbreeding are well-documented in animals and plants, and notably lead to a decrease in offspring birth weight, survival, fecundity, low resistance to pathogens, reduction in behavioural and morphological sexual traits or an increased susceptibility to predation or environmental stress (reviewed by [2], [3]). These impacts of inbreeding depression are largely context-dependent. They are greater in stressful than benign environments [4]. They also depend on the genetic diversity and inbreeding history of the study population, which can induce strong differences between species or populations [5][7]. For example, outbred populations of Drosophila melanogaster that experienced ancestral inbreeding were able to purge the deleterious alleles thanks to natural selection, which reduced the level of inbreeding depression [6][7].

Dispersal and kin recognition are therefore assumed to have evolved to avoid inbred mating [8], [9], and evidence of inbreeding avoidance via mate choice or preference for genetically dissimilar mate is plentiful (e.g. [9][24]). However, inbreeding might be as disadvantageous as outbreeding (i.e. mating with a genetically distant individual), due to the break down of locally adapted or co-adapted gene complexes [25], [11]. Therefore it has been suggested that individuals should prefer mating with individuals of intermediate relatedness to balance costs of both inbreeding and outbreeding [26][29].

Recently, Kokko & Ots [17] and Puurtinen [30] shed a new light on inbreeding avoidance by revisiting Hamilton’s inclusive fitness theory [31], initially developed to explain social evolution and eusociality. Also known as kin selection theory, this theory postulates that the inclusive fitness of an organism is equal to the sum of the increments in individual fitness of all relatives (including self) weighted by their relatedness to the organism [31]. Thus a female mating with a related male will gain direct fitness benefits from this mating (through her offspring), and indirect fitness by increasing the mating success of her relative mate. One condition is that such mating does not reduce mating opportunities for the male. As a consequence, inclusive benefits of mating with a relative are positively linked to relatedness between mates. These benefits are also higher in species in which both males and females mate multiply, as both genders will gain extra inclusive fitness from inbred mating. Using mathematical models, Kokko & Ots [17] and Puurtinen [30] showed that inbreeding avoidance is highly context-dependent. Inbreeding tolerance or even preference for related mates are likely to evolve in specific conditions, even if inbreeding depression imposes substantial costs, and thus should be more prevalent than previously appreciated. For example, it was predicted that sequential mate choice should favour inbreeding tolerance compared to simultaneous mate choice when mate availability is low (at least for one of the sexes), and that the sex that provides higher parental investment (usually males) should have higher inbreeding tolerance than the other sex [17]. Generally, the species expected to show the highest inbreeding tolerance are those in which mate availability is low and both males and females are able to breed multiple times [17].

If advantages of inbreeding tolerance can outweigh its costs under some circumstances, why is empirical evidence of inbreeding tolerance so limited? Kokko & Ots [17] suggested, among other explanations, that evidence would exist if we looked for it. Interestingly, since then, the list of examples of inbreeding tolerance is growing (e.g. [2], [32][41]). However, experimental evidence of mating preferences for close relatives is still scarce (e.g. bird [26]; cestode [42]; insects [3]; fish [43]). Experimental tests represent the best way to explore inbreeding avoidance and tolerance. Indeed, investigations in wild populations may not always provide clear-cut results. For example, genetic data revealed non-random mating with respect to relatedness in a natural population of D. melanogaster, as males appeared more likely to be mating with a related female, but this finding may result from an overall elevated relatedness between all sampled individuals (not just between mating pairs) ([44] see also [45] for a similar issue), and it is still not clear whether this finding results from female or male mate choice. Robinson and colleagues [44] therefore concluded that an experimental approach exploring mate preferences with respect to relatedness is needed in this species.

Here, we investigated the strength and the direction of mate preference for relatedness in both male and female D. melanogaster. D. melanogaster is an ideal species to examine inbreeding avoidance and tolerance. Both males and females mate multiply [46]. Experimental isofemale lines are generated by mating first-order relatives (full-siblings), showing that a high relatedness does not preclude reproduction and maintenance of highly inbred populations in laboratory conditions. Moreover, a field study revealed potential male preference for related mates, with lower number of offspring produced as an associated cost of inbreeding [44]. Additional costs of inbreeding may be a reduced sperm competitive ability [47] (but see [48][49] for contrasting results). Finally, short generation time, capacity to control genetic relatedness and possibility to observe mate choice in the laboratory make D. melanogaster an ideal species to examine the effects of genetic relatedness on mate choice.

We performed laboratory mate choice experiments in which individuals were given simultaneous access to two individuals of the opposite sex, one related (full-sibling or parent) and one unrelated (with similar age and mating experience). We therefore tested four types of mate choice: 1) female choice between a brother and a non-brother male, 2) female choice between the father and a non-father male, 3) male choice between a sister and a non-sister female and 4) male choice between the mother and a non-mother female.


Materials and Methods
Culture Stocks and Generation of Experimental Males and Females

We used D. melanogaster of the wild type strain obtained from a large outbred laboratory population with overlapping generations. This population was derived from 2000 flies caught in Chavroches (France) in 2006 and was kindly provided by F. Méry in 2008. The flies were maintained at 20°C on a natural light:dark cycle, at low density and in standard 8 ml vials containing cornmeal-agar-yeast medium. All the flies were manipulated by gentle aspiration and were not anaesthetized.

To create 115 families from the stocks, we obtained virgin flies by separating males and females at emergence. Three days later, we randomly placed one female and one male into a 1 ml vial. The vials where no copulation occurred within 30 minutes were discarded. After copulation, each male (the father) and each female (the mother) were kept separately in an individual 8 ml vial containing food. Females were transferred on new vials containing food every four days. Resulting male and female offspring were sexed at emergence and placed separately in vials containing food. They were then used to test mate preferences 3 or 4 days later.

Mate Choice Test

We aimed to investigate four types of mate choice: 1) female choice between a brother and a non-brother male, 2) female choice between the father and a non-father male, 3) male choice between a sister and a non-sister female and 4) male choice between the mother and a non-mother female. Choices for parent vs. unrelated individual were tested as early as possible, i.e. with offspring resulting from the first emergences to reduce parental age at experiment. This resulted in mothers and fathers being respectively 21±1 and 26±1 days old.

Tests were performed daily from 09:30 to 18:30. We used an experimental set up consisting of three transparent plastic vials (3.3 cm long and 1 cm diameter) separated by two removable microscope cover glasses, providing three compartments (as in [50]). Vials were cleaned with absolute ethanol and new glass partitions were used for each replicate. We placed the choosing individual (male or female) in the central compartment and one individual of the opposite sex in each peripheral compartment. We then removed the glass partitions and observed free interactions.

The focal individual had the choice between one related individual (either sibling or parent), and one unrelated individual of similar age and mating experience. Pairs of related and unrelated individuals were randomly chosen among every possible pair of families. All individuals involved in the mate choice test were used only once, but we sometimes tested mate choice of several male offspring and several female offspring of a given family. When this occurred we avoided full pseudo-replication by choosing the unrelated flies in another family.

To distinguish between same-sex individuals, flies were marked with either a green or a pink colour powder as in [50][51], by introducing them into a vial containing traces of powder. They were then transferred into a clean vial containing food where they stayed for at least 45 min before experiment. This delay allowed them to clean so that only few colour spots dusted their thorax and back at the onset of the experiment. Coloration had no impact on mate choice (GLM, female choice: Khi21,220 = 0.00, p = 1.00; male choice: Khi21,53 = 1.80, p = 0.1794). Related and unrelated individuals were alternately placed in the left or right peripheral compartments, and were alternately green or pink.

We measured mate preference by recording the identity (colour) of the chosen individual i.e. the individual who started to copulate with the focal individual. We also recorded the time from the removal of the partition glasses until copulation (latency to copulation). Male Drosophila intensively court virgin females, as a result females are expected to mate faster with more attractive males [52]. Because up to ten set ups were observed in parallel, the precise time of the beginning or end of the copulation could not be observed for some replicates, which accounts for discrepancies in the number of replicates between Fig. 1 and Fig. 2. Replicates in which no copulation occurred within 1 h were discarded from further analyses.

Data Analyses

Statistical analyses were performed using SAS 9.1.3 (Cary, USA). We investigated mate choice for related versus unrelated individuals with generalized linear mixed models (GLMM, proc glimmix) with a binomial distribution of error terms (0/1 = unsuccessful/successful, link function: logit). The identity of the focal individual and the family identities were set as random factors to account for repeated sampling. Latency to copulation was investigated using generalized linear models (GLM, proc genmod) with a normal or Poisson distribution of effort terms (link functions: identity and log) and identity of the focal individual and family identities as repeated subjects. Represented values are means ± standard errors. Standard errors of frequencies were calculated as the following: s.e. = √[p(1-p)/n] where p and 1-p are the proportions and n the number of replicates.


Results

We performed 167 replicates of brother vs. non-brother choice, 142 replicates of the daughter vs. non-daughter choice, and 54 replicates of the father vs. non-father choice during which copulation occurred. We also did 20 replicates of the mother vs. non-mother choice, in which all males were observed courting the 21±1 days old females, but no successful copulation occurred. This absence of copulation suggests that these females were still refractory at the time of experiment, i.e. 18 days after their first mating, and/or too aged to mate. This length for a refractory period is plausible in view of previous results [53]. Family identity was never significant (all ps>0.05).

When females were given the choice between a brother and an unrelated male, they mated significantly more often with their bothers (GLMM, F1,166 = 16.05, p<0.0001; Fig. 1). However, when they copulated with unrelated males, the latency to copulation was significantly lower compared to when they copulated with their brothers (GLM, d.f. = 1, Khi2 = 7.26, p = 0.0071; Fig. 2). When given the choice between a sister and an unrelated female, males tolerated mating with their sisters, as they did not show any preference between the two potential female mates (GLMM, F1,141 = 0.4797; Fig. 1). Male latency to copulation was comparable when mating to a sister and to an unrelated female (GLM, d.f. = 1, Khi2 = 0.14, p = 0.7128; Fig. 2). Similarly, females tolerated mating with their fathers as they did not show any preference between the two proposed males (GLMM, F1,53 = 0.15, p = 0.7019; Fig. 1) and latency to copulation did not differ between fathers and unrelated males (GLM, d.f. = 1, Khi2 = 0.36, p = 0.5458; Fig. 2).


Discussion

We found evidence that D. melanogaster adult females preferred mating with their brothers over unrelated males with similar age and mating experience, which supports inbreeding preference in this species. Moreover, females tolerated mating with their fathers, and males tolerated mating with their sisters when they were given the choice between their father or their sister, and an unrelated individual, which supports inbreeding tolerance [17]. We also tried to investigate male mate choice between their mother and an unrelated female, but while males courted both females no copulation occurred (n = 20 replicates). Despite this relatively small number of replicates, the absence of copulation suggests that females were still refractory or aged and that such a mating is thus unlikely in nature implying that there may be no real selective pressure on such kind of choice.

Our experimental results confirms the non-random mating pattern with respect to genetic similarity found by Robinson et al. [44] in a natural population of D. melanogaster. It provides an additional experimental example of mate preference for inbreeding to the few ones that exist so far [3], [26], [42][43], and strengthens the prediction that inbreeding tolerance and mate preference for inbreeding are likely to evolve, as predicted through the positive effect on inclusive fitness [17], [30]. A female mating with her brother increases her inclusive fitness, while not restricting the mating opportunities of her brother because D. melanogaster males mate multiply [46]. On the contrary, copulation induces a refractory period during which the female cannot gain additional mating. This dissymmetry may explain why females, and not males, prefer mating with relatives in this species.

Despite the overall preference for mating with brothers, there was substantial variation among females when testing for the brother vs. unrelated choice. Interestingly, the latency to copulation was significantly lower for copulation with an unrelated male than with the brother. Latency to copulation is a good indicator of male attractiveness in this species [52], therefore our result suggests that females who preferred unrelated males over their brothers did so strategically and not for lack of a better option. One major explanation may be that, in D. melanogaster, female mate choice is known to be influenced by many morphological and behavioural male traits, including body size, comb size, courtship vigour, song frequency or pheromone production [54][56]. In our experiments, the unrelated males who were preferred over brothers may also have been of particular high quality with respect to these traits. In addition although the coefficient of relatedness for siblings is 0.5 on average, it can theoretically range from 0 to 1. Hence, females may simply copulate with unrelated males rather than with their brothers in those instances in which brothers and sisters were far too genetically similar.

If benefits in terms of inclusive fitness are sufficient to explain why D. melanogaster females prefer mating with their brothers, it is not clear why females did not also prefer mating with their fathers. This difference in mate preference is unexpected and somewhat surprising. One possibility is that, in our experiment, females could not discriminate their fathers from unrelated males. The probability that an individual meets its father or mother may be negligible in nature, thus there might be no selective pressure for such parent-offspring recognition. However, preference for relatives requires a mechanism of kin recognition. Given that females were able to recognise their brothers, we could reasonably suppose that they could also recognise their fathers. Several mechanisms have been proposed so far: family phenotype matching that implies that related individuals are used as references to perform a generalization against unrelated individuals, self-referent phenotype matching in which an individual uses its own phenotype as a reference against unrelated individuals, and recognition alleles that allow intrinsic recognition without learning (reviewed by [57]). In social and non-social insects, cuticular hydrocarbons (CHCs) are thought to play an important role in kin recognition [44]. A recent study in D. melanogaster unravelled that CHC profiles are strongly altered by aging, with the expression of long-chain CHCs increasing as individuals get older [58]. These changes of individual CHCs with age do not reflect a general deterioration in the capacity to produce individual CHCs because overall CHC levels increased [58]. However, they may affect a female’s ability to detect relatedness. Another possibility is that females did discriminate between fathers and unrelated males but based their mate choice on male characteristics other than relatedness, as already suggested above. Females are normally reluctant to mate with old males because courtship and mating abilities decline with age [58][60]. As males may exhibit inter-individual variation in the way they suffer from aging, females may use alternative male characteristics to do the best of a bad job, when having to choose between two old males.

One intriguing result of the mathematical model developed by Kokko & Ots [17] is that inbreeding tolerance and/or preference can be selected even when imposing substantial costs in terms of inbreeding depression, which is supported by empirical work [40], [43]. In our study, we did not measure the fitness outcome of the observed mating and thus we could not evaluate the direct costs of inbreeding. Whether observed mate preference may cause inbreeding depression in D. melanogaster is not clear yet. Robinson and colleagues [44], [61] found a negative relationship between the degree of genetic similarity within a pair and both egg-to-adult-viability and number offspring at emergence. Increased relatedness also decreased male ability to secure a mating when competing with non-inbred males [62]. However, in a population experiencing an intermediate level of inbreeding, as we may expect from our observations, a purging effect of deleterious alleles is likely to lower the impact of inbreeding, as experimentally found in D. melanogaster[63]. Our results were obtained from an experiment conducted with of a single population of D. melanogaster. As the impacts of inbreeding depression largely depend on environmental conditions and the genetic history of the population [4][7], further studies should investigate mate choice in regards to inbreeding in various environmental and genetic contexts. Populations that went through bottleneck(s) may have purged most deleterious alleles responsible for inbreeding depression and therefore be more prone to inbreeding tolerance or even preference, on the one hand. One the other hand, the loss of genetic diversity following bottlenecks may make kin recognition arduous, not allowing the evolution of inbreeding preferences.

To summarize, we performed experimental mate choice tests in the laboratory and found that D. melanogaster females prefer mating with their brothers over unrelated males. This result provides strong support for inbreeding preference, as benefits from inclusive fitness may outweigh the potential costs of inbreeding depression [17]. We also found that females tolerated mating with their fathers and that males tolerated mating with their sisters, which supports inbreeding tolerance.


We are especially grateful to Pierre Vincent for his help in gathering preliminary data, to Frédéric Méry for kindly providing the coloured powders and the flies, and to Hanna Kokko and Susanne RK Zajitschek for insightful comments that greatly improved the manuscript.


References
1. Charlesworth D,, Charlesworth B, (Year: 1987) Inbreeding depression and its evolutionary consequences. Ann Rev Ecol Systematics18: 237–268
2. Guevara-Fiore P,, Rosenqvist G,, Watt PJ, (Year: 2010) Inbreeding level does not induce females discrimination between sibs and unrelated males in guppies. Behav Ecol Sociobiol64: 1601–1607
3. Thurin N,, Aron S, (Year: 2010) Sib-mating in the ant Plagiolepis pygmaea: adaptative inbreeding?J Evol Biol22: 2481–248719878410
4. Fox CW,, Reed DH, (Year: 2011) Inbreeding depression increases with environmental stress: an experimental study and meta-analysis. Evolution65(1): 246–25820731715
5. Frankham R, Ballou JD, Briscoe DA (2002) Introduction to Conservation Genetics. University Press, Cambridge.
6. Swindell WR,, Bouzat JL, (Year: 2006) Reduced inbreeding depression due to historical inbreeding in Drosophila melanogaster: evidence for purging. J Evol Biol19: 1257–126416780526
7. Swindell WR,, Bouzat JL, (Year: 2006) Ancestral inbreeding reduces the magnitude of inbreeding depression in Drosophila melanogaster. Evolution60(4): 762–76716739457
8. Blouin SF,, Blouin M, (Year: 1988) Inbreeding avoidance behaviors. Trends Ecol Evol3: 230–23321227237
9. Pusey A,, Wolf M, (Year: 1996) Inbreeding avoidance in animals. Trends Ecol Evol11: 201–20621237809
10. Koenig WD,, Stanback MT,, Haydock J, (Year: 1999) Demographic consequences of incest avoidance in the cooperatively breeding acorn woodpecker. Anim Behav57: 1287–129310373262
11. Trezenga T,, Wedell N, (Year: 2000) Genetic compatibility, mate choice and patterns of parentage. Mol Ecol9: 1013–102710964221
12. Daniels SJ,, Walters JR, (Year: 2000) Inbreeding depression and its effects on natal dispersal in red-cockaded woodpeckers. Condor102: 482–491
13. Hatchwell BJ,, Russell AF,, Ross DJ,, Fowlie MK, (Year: 2000) Divorce in cooperatively breeding long-tailed tits: a consequence of inbreeding avoidance?Proc R Soc Lond B267: 813–819
14. Blomqvist D,, Andersson M,, Kupper C,, Cuthill IC,, Kis J,, et al. (Year: 2002) Genetic similarity between mates and extra-pair parentage in three species of shorebirds. Nature419: 613–61512374978
15. Freeman-Gallant CR,, Meguerdichian M,, Wheelwright NT,, Sollecito SV, (Year: 2003) Social pairing and female mating fidelity predicted by restriction fragment length polymorphism similarity at the major histocompatibility complex in a song bird. Mol Ecol12: 3077–308314629387
16. Olsson M,, Madsen T,, Nordbly J,, Wapstra E,, Ujvari B,, et al. (Year: 2003) Major histocompatibility complex and mate choice in sand lizards. Proc. R. Soc. Lond B270: S254–S256
17. Kokko H,, Ots I, (Year: 2006) When not to avoid inbreeding. Evolution60: 467–47516637492
18. Frommen JG,, Bakker TCM, (Year: 2006) Inbreeding avoidance through non-random mating in sticklebacks. Biol Lett2: 232–23517148370
19. Gerlach G,, Lysiak N, (Year: 2006) Kin recognition and inbreeding avoidance in zebrafish, Danio rerio, is based on phenotype matching. Anim Behav71: 1371–1377
20. Wheelwright NT,, Freeman-Gallant CR,, Mauck RA, (Year: 2006) Asymmetrical incest avoidance in the choice of social and genetic mates. Anim Behav71: 631–639
21. Hoffman JI,, Forcada J,, Trathan PN,, Amos B, (Year: 2007) Female fur seals show active choice for males that are heterozygous and unrelated. Nature445: 912–91417287726
22. Lieberman D,, Tooby J,, Cosmides L, (Year: 2007) The architecture of human kin detection. Nature445: 727–73117301784
23. Mulard H,, Danchin É,, Talbot SL,, Ramey AM,, Hatch SA,, et al. (Year: 2009) Evidence that pairing with genetically similar mates is maladaptative in a monogamous bird. BMC Evol Biol9: 14719566922
24. Lihoreau M,, Zimmer C,, Rivault C, (Year: 2010) Mutual mate choice: when it pays both sexes to avoid inbreeding. PLoS ONE3: e336518843373
25. Parker C (1979) Sexual selection and sexual conflict. In: Blum MS, Blum NA, editors. Sexual selection and reproductive competition in insects. New York Academic Press. 123–166.
26. Bateson P, (Year: 1982) Preferences for cousins in Japanese quail. Nature295: 236–237
27. Bateson P (1983) Optimal outbreeding. In: Bateson P, editor. Mate choice. Cambridge University Press. 257–277.
28. Barnard CJ,, Fitzsimons J, (Year: 1988) Kin recognition and mate choice in mice – the effects of kinship, familiarity and social interference on intersexual interaction. Anim Behav36: 1078–1090
29. Keane B, (Year: 1990) The effect of relateness on reproductive success and mate choice in the white-footed mouse, Peromyscus leucopus. Anim Behav39: 264–273
30. Puurtinen M, (Year: 2011) Mate choice for optimal (k)inbreeding. Evolution65: 1501–150521521199
31. Hamilton WD, (Year: 1964) Genetical evolution of social behaviour I. J Theor Biol. 7: 1–52
32. Hansson B,, Jack L,, Christians JK,, Pemberton JM,, Akesson M,, et al. (Year: 2007) No evidence for inbreeding avoidance in a great reed warbler population. Behav Ecol18: 157–164
33. Holand O,, Askim KR,, Røed KH,, Weladji RB,, Gjøstein H,, et al. (Year: 2007) No evidence of inbreeding avoidance in a polygynous ungulate: the reindeer (Rangifer tarandus). Biol Lett3: 36–3917443960
34. De Luca PA,, Cocroft RB, (Year: 2008) The effects of age and relatedness on mating patterns in thornbug treehoppers: inbreeding avoidance or inbreeding tolerance?Behav Ecol Sociobiol62: 1869–1875
35. Edvardsson M,, Rodriguez-Munoz R,, Tregenza T, (Year: 2008) No evidence that female bruchid beetles, Callosobruchus maculatus, use remating to reduce costs of inbreeding. Anim Behav75: 1519–1524
36. Jamieson IG,, Taylor SS,, Tracy LN,, Kokko H,, Armstrong DP, (Year: 2009) Why some species of birds do not avoid inbreeding: insights from New Zealand robins and saddlebacks. Behav Ecol20: 575–584
37. Szulkin M,, Zelazowski P,, Nicholson G,, Sheldon BC, (Year: 2009) Inbreeding avoidance under different null models of random mating in the great tit. J Anim Ecol78: 778–78819383076
38. Richard M,, Losdat S,, Lecomte J,, de Fraipont M,, Clobert J, (Year: 2009) Optimal level of inbreeding in the common lizard. Proc R Soc Lond B276: 2779–2786
39. Frère CH,, Krützen M,, Kopps AM,, Ward P,, Mann J,, et al. (Year: 2010) Inbreeding tolerance and fitness costs in wild bottlenose dolphins. Proc R Soc Lond B277: 2667–2673
40. Elias J,, Dorn S,, Mazzi D, (Year: 2010) Inbreeding in a natural population of the gregarious parasitoid wasp Cotesia glomerata. Mol Ecol19: 2336–234520465585
41. Rioux-Paquette E,, Festa-Bianchet M,, Coltman DW, (Year: 2010) No inbreeding avoidance in an isolated population of bighorn sheep. Anim Behav80: 865–871
42. Schjørring S,, Jäger I, (Year: 2007) Incestuous mate preference by a simultaneous hermaphrodite with strong inbreeding depression. Evolution61: 423–43017348951
43. Thünken T,, Bakker TCM,, Baldauf SA,, Kullmann H, (Year: 2007) Active inbreeding in a cichlid fish and its adaptive significance. Curr Biol17: 225–22917276915
44. Robinson SP,, Kennington WJ,, Simmons LW, (Year: 2012) Assortative mating for relatedness in a large naturally occurring population of Drosophila melanogaster. J Evol Biol25: 716–72522321157
45. Reid JM,, Arcese P,, Keller LF, (Year: 2006) Intrinsic parent-offspring correlation in inbreeding level in a song sparrow (Melospiza melodia) population open to immigration. Am Nat168: 1–13
46. Jones B,, Clark AG, (Year: 2003) Bayesian sperm competition estimates. Genetics163: 1193–119912663555
47. Mack PD,, Hammock BA,, Promislow DEL, (Year: 2002) Sperm competitive ability and genetic relatedness in Drosophila melanogaster: similarity breeds contempt. Evolution56: 1789–179512389723
48. Clark AG,, Begun DJ,, Prout T, (Year: 1999) Female x male interactions in Drosophila sperm competition. Science283: 217–2209880253
49. Ala-Honkola O,, Manier MK,, Lupold S,, Pitnick S, (Year: 2011) No evidence for postcopulatory inbreeding avoidance in Drosophila melanogaster. Evolution65: 2699–270521884066
50. Loyau A,, Blanchet S,, Van Laere P,, Clobert J,, Danchin É, (Year: 2012) When not to copy: female fruit flies use sophisticated public information to avoid mated males. Sci Rep2: 76823105967
51. Méry F,, Varela SAM,, Danchin É,, Blanchet S,, Parejo D,, et al. (Year: 2009) Public versus personal information for mate choice copying in an invertebrate. Curr Biol19: 1–1519135370
52. Narraway C,, Hunt J,, Wedell N,, Hosken DJ, (Year: 2010) Genotype-by-environment interactions for female preference. J Evol Biol23: 2550–255720939836
53. Yew JY,, Dreisewerd K,, Luftmann H,, Müthing J,, Pohlentz G,, et al. (Year: 2009) A new male sex pheromone and novel cuticular cues for chemical communication in Drosophila. Curr Biol19: 1245–125419615904
54. Partridge L,, Ewing A,, Chandler A, (Year: 1987) Male size and mating success in Drosophila melanogaster: the roles of male and female behaviour. Anim Behav35: 555–562
55. Tomaru M,, Oguma Y, (Year: 2000) Mate choice in Drosophila melanogaster and D. sechellia: criteria and their variation depending on courtship song. Anim Behav60: 797–80411124878
56. Ahuja A,, Singh RS, (Year: 2008) Variation and evolution of male sex combs in Drosophila: nature of selection response and theories of genetic variation for sexual traits. Genetics179: 503–50918493067
57. Schielzeth H,, Burger C,, Bolund E,, Forstmeier W, (Year: 2008) Assortative versus disassortative mating preferences of female zebra finches based on self-referent phenotype matching. Anim Behav76: 1927–1934
58. Kuo T-H,, Yew JY,, Fedina TY,, Dreisewerd K,, et al. (Year: 2012) Aging modulates cuticular hydrocarbons and sexual attractiveness in Drosophila melanogaster. J Exp Biol215: 814–82122323204
59. Mack PD,, Lester VK,, Promislow DEL, (Year: 2000) Age-specific effects of novel mutations in Drosophila melanogaster – II. Fecundity and male mating ability. Genetica110: 31–4111519873
60. Ruedi EA,, Hughes KA, (Year: 2009) Age, but not experience, affects courtship gene expression in male Drosophila melanogaster. PLoS ONE4(7): e615019582156
61. Robinson SP,, Kennington WJ,, Simmons LW, (Year: 2009) No evidence for optimal fitness at intermediate levels of inbreeding in Drosophila melanogaster. Biol J Lin Soc98: 501–510
62. Sharp PM, (Year: 1984) The effect of inbreeding on competitive male-mating ability in Drosophila melanogaster. Genetics106: 601–61217246200
63. Ávila V,, Amador C,, García-Dorado A, (Year: 2010) The purge of genetic load through restricted panmixia in a Drosophila experiment. J Evol Biol23: 1937–194620695969

Figures

[Figure ID: pone-0051293-g001]
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0051293.g001.
Figure 1  Copulation frequency for brother vs. non-brother, sister vs. non-sister and father vs. non-father.

No copulation occurred when we tested mother vs. non-mother.



[Figure ID: pone-0051293-g002]
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0051293.g002.
Figure 2  Latency to copulation for brother vs. non-brother, sister vs. non-sister and father vs. non-father.

No copulation occurred when we tested mother vs. non-mother.



Article Categories:
  • Research Article
Article Categories:
  • Biology
    • Ecology
      • Behavioral Ecology
    • Evolutionary Biology
      • Evolutionary Processes
        • Sexual Selection
      • Animal Behavior
      • Evolutionary Ecology
    • Model Organisms
      • Animal Models
        • Drosophila Melanogaster
    • Zoology
      • Animal Behavior


Previous Document:  Characterisation and validation of insertions and deletions in 173 patient exomes.
Next Document:  Endogenous repair by the activation of cell survival signalling cascades during the early stages of ...