Document Detail

Eating behaviour and weight in children.
Jump to Full Text
MedLine Citation:
PMID:  19002146     Owner:  NLM     Status:  MEDLINE    
Abstract/OtherAbstract:
OBJECTIVE: To test the hypothesis that quantitative variation in eating behaviour traits shows a graded association with weight in children.
DESIGN: Cross-sectional design in a community setting.
SUBJECTS: Data were from 406 families participating in the Physical Exercise and Appetite in CHildren Study (PEACHES) or the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS). Children were aged 7-9 years (PEACHES) and 9-12 years (TEDS).
MEASUREMENTS: Weights and heights were measured by researchers. Body mass index (BMI) s.d. scores were used to categorize participants into underweight, healthy weight, overweight and obese groups, with an additional division of the healthy weight group into higher and lower healthy weight at the 50th centile. Eating behaviour traits were assessed with the Child Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (CEBQ), completed by the parents on behalf of their child. Linear trend analyses compared CEBQ subscale scores across the five weight groups.
RESULTS: Satiety Responsiveness/Slowness in Eating and Food Fussiness showed a graded negative association with weight, whereas Food Responsiveness, Enjoyment of Food, Emotional Overeating and Desire to Drink were positively associated. All effects were maintained after controlling for age, sex, ethnicity, parental education and sample. There was no systematic association with weight for Emotional Undereating.
CONCLUSION: These results support the idea that approach-related and avoidance-related appetitive traits are systematically (and oppositely) related to adiposity, and not exclusively associated with obesity. Early assessment of these traits could be used as indicators of susceptibility to weight gain.
Authors:
L Webber; C Hill; J Saxton; C H M Van Jaarsveld; J Wardle
Related Documents :
15379416 - Oxidative stress in childhood obesity.
12891156 - Prevention of childhood obesity: sociocultural and familial factors.
16922816 - Obesity in childhood and cardiovascular risk.
16430316 - Altering tv viewing habits: an unexplored strategy for adult obesity intervention?
11888416 - Challenges in long-term health care for children.
15662156 - The effectiveness of risperidone in the treatment of three children with feeding disord...
Publication Detail:
Type:  Journal Article; Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't; Twin Study     Date:  2008-11-11
Journal Detail:
Title:  International journal of obesity (2005)     Volume:  33     ISSN:  1476-5497     ISO Abbreviation:  Int J Obes (Lond)     Publication Date:  2009 Jan 
Date Detail:
Created Date:  2009-01-12     Completed Date:  2009-09-16     Revised Date:  2013-03-27    
Medline Journal Info:
Nlm Unique ID:  101256108     Medline TA:  Int J Obes (Lond)     Country:  England    
Other Details:
Languages:  eng     Pagination:  21-8     Citation Subset:  IM    
Affiliation:
Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College London, Cancer Research UK Health Behaviour Research Centre, 2-16 Torrington Place, Gower Street, London, UK.
Export Citation:
APA/MLA Format     Download EndNote     Download BibTex
MeSH Terms
Descriptor/Qualifier:
Adiposity
Age Factors
Appetite Regulation
Body Mass Index
Child
Cross-Sectional Studies
Diseases in Twins / ethnology,  psychology*
Educational Status
Emotions
Ethnic Groups
Feeding Behavior*
Female
Health Surveys
Humans
Male
Obesity / ethnology,  psychology*
Regression Analysis
Sex Factors
Thirst
Twins
Grant Support
ID/Acronym/Agency:
31/D19086//Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council; A7974//Cancer Research UK; G0500079//Medical Research Council; //Cancer Research UK
Comments/Corrections

From MEDLINE®/PubMed®, a database of the U.S. National Library of Medicine

Full Text
Journal Information
Journal ID (nlm-journal-id): 101256108
Journal ID (pubmed-jr-id): 32579
Journal ID (nlm-ta): Int J Obes (Lond)
Journal ID (iso-abbrev): Int J Obes (Lond)
ISSN: 0307-0565
ISSN: 1476-5497
Article Information
Download PDF

nihms-submitted publication date: Day: 26 Month: 1 Year: 2010
Electronic publication date: Day: 11 Month: 11 Year: 2008
Print publication date: Month: 1 Year: 2009
pmc-release publication date: Day: 08 Month: 2 Year: 2010
Volume: 33 Issue: 1
First Page: 21 Last Page: 28
PubMed Id: 19002146
ID: 2817450
DOI: 10.1038/ijo.2008.219
ID: UKMS2601

Eating Behaviour and Weight in Children
L Webber1
C Hill1
J Saxton1
CHM Van Jaarsveld1
J Wardle1
1Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College London, London, UK
Correspondence: Address correspondence to Jane Wardle, Cancer Research UK Health Behaviour Research Centre, Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, UCL, Gower Street, London, WC1E 6BT, UK. Tel: +44207 6796642; fax: +44(0)207 8132848; j.wardle@ucl.ac.uk

Introduction

Research into the behavioural correlates of obesity has identified a variety of eating behaviour traits in obese children and adults (1-3). In the paediatric literature, behavioural studies have shown that obese children have lower responsiveness to internal satiety signals (4-6), eat faster during the course of a meal (7-9) and are more sensitive to external food cues (10) than healthy-weight children. Heightened desire to consume may go beyond food to include palatable drinks, with intake of high-energy drinks being associated with weight gain (11). Food fussiness (or pickiness) has also been linked with weight, with picky girls having lower BMI and lower rates of overweight (12;13), although this finding has not always been replicated (14-16).

Ingestive responses to stress or distress have also been proposed as significant influences on food intake. The idea originated in the ‘psychosomatic theory’ of obesity, which proposed that eating in response to emotional distress rather than hunger was one cause of excessive weight gain (17), while more recent analyses suggest that stress can deregulate eating through effects on the reward system (18). In practice, empirical studies analysing associations between food intake, weight and stress have not produced entirely consistent results (19;20). However, one recent study in young adults found that those who had either gained or lost weight reported more stress than those whose weights were stable (21), suggesting that individuals may vary in the extent to which they experience hyperphagic or hypophagic responses to emotional distress.

It has recently been argued that appetitive traits are not only pathogenic for obesity per se, but vary quantitatively across the entire weight distribution and could determine risk of positive energy balance (1;2). This is consistent with evidence that the same genetic influences determine variation in weight within the healthy range as determine the extremes of obesity (22). Positive responses to food – enjoyment, responsiveness to palatability and rapid consumption - are hypothesised to promote food intake, while sensitivity to internal cues of satiety or fussiness about food are likely to reduce intake. In environments with multiple opportunities to eat highly palatable, energy-dense foods, these appetitive traits will moderate the risk of weight gain.

One recent study used the Child Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (CEBQ) (23) to index a number of positive and negative appetitive traits, and compared obese/overweight and healthy-weight children, as well as a small sample of underweight children (24). Consistent with previous work, obese/overweight children showed higher responsiveness to food cues, more emotional eating, lower satiety responsiveness and less fussiness than healthy-weight children. In addition, the underweight group differed from the healthy-weight group, with lower responsiveness to food cues, lower emotional eating, higher satiety responsiveness and greater fussiness. These results supported the idea of a graded association between appetite and weight, rather than an aberrant eating style that is specific to clinically overweight populations, although the authors noted that the small sample size for the underweight group, and the different recruitment methods for overweight/obese and healthy-weight/underweight children, limited the confidence with which conclusions could be drawn. However, a very similar association was reported in a recent community-based study, although this study assessed only two aspects of appetite, Satiety Responsiveness and Enjoyment of Food (1).

In the present study we examined associations with weight for the seven appetitive traits included in the CEBQ scale in a large, community-based, sample of 7 to 12-year-old children. To examine the patterning of associations between appetite and weight in more detail, we not only compared obese, overweight, healthy-weight and underweight groups, but also subdivided the healthy-weight group into higher-healthy-weight and lower-healthy-weight at the 50th centile. We predicted that CEBQ sub-scales indicating ‘food approach’ (Enjoyment of Food, Food Responsiveness, Emotional Overeating, Desire to Drink) would show a positive association with weight, while Satiety Responsiveness/Slowness in Eating, Food Fussiness and Emotional Under-eating would be highest in the thinnest children and show a negative association with weight.


Method
Participants

The data for these analyses came from two community-based samples. One sample consisted of 7 to 9-year-old children and their parents recruited through five schools around London as part of the Physical Exercise and Appetite in CHildren (PEACHES) study (Sample 1). This is a longitudinal study examining associations between eating behaviour, physical activity and adiposity during childhood, and the data reported here come from the first year of the study. Out of a total of 531 families invited to take part, 400 parents (206 parents of boys and 194 parents of girls), replied to the invitation giving consent to their child's participation. Parents were sent a questionnaire containing the CEBQ and socio-demographic questions (including education level and ethnicity), of which 268 were returned, a response rate of 67%.

The second sample was from a sub-sample of 214 families from the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS) – a cohort study beginning when the twins were 4 years-old (25), enrolled in a longitudinal study of eating behaviour and weight. Of these, 100 families had overweight or obese parents and 114 had normal-weight parents. Families were matched on social class. 173 (81%) families participated in the 2006 follow-up. The data here come from one child per family selected at random from the follow-up when the children were aged 9 to 12 years. Children were weighed and measured and parents were asked to complete the CEBQ and questions relating to their socio-economic status (including education level and ethnicity) during a study home visit.

Psychometric measures

The CEBQ is 35-item validated instrument designed to assess a range of eating behaviours in children (23). It has good internal consistency, test-retest reliability, and stability over time (23;26) and has been shown to be related to food intake in behavioural tests (27). It includes four sub-scales that measure food approach behaviours (Enjoyment of Food, Food Responsiveness, Emotional Overeating, Desire to Drink) and four that index more avoidant-type responses (Satiety Responsiveness/Slowness in Eating, Emotional Under-eating, Food Fussiness). Because Satiety Responsiveness and Slowness in Eating have been found to load onto the same factor (23) they were combined to form a single sub-scale. Response options for all sub-scales were from ‘never’ to ‘always’ on a 1-5 likert scale.

Anthropometrics

In both samples, a stadiometer (Leicester height measure, Seca, Birmingham, UK) was used to measure children's height to the nearest millimetre. Children were weighed to the nearest tenth of a kilogram using Tanita TBF-300MA Body Composition Analyser in sample 1 and a digital Tanita scale in sample 2 (Tanita Corporation, Tokyo, Japan). In both samples, waist circumference was measured using standard instructions (28).

BMI was calculated from children's weight and height and the Imsgrowth macro (http://homepage.mac.com/tjcole) was used to transform BMI into age and sex appropriate BMI SD-scores from British 1990 reference data (29). BMI SD-scores were categorised according to recommended groupings for underweight (thinness grade 1, 2, 3; (30)) and IOTF (International Obesity Taskforce) categories for healthy-weight, overweight and obese (31). The healthy-weight group was subdivided into lower-healthy-weight (≤50th centile) and higher-healthy-weight (>50th centile but not meeting criteria for overweight) for comparison of eating behaviours across the weight spectrum.

Statistical analysis

Relationships between possible covariates (ethnicity, education, age, sex) and BMI SD-score were assessed using Pearson's correlations and t-tests where appropriate. Mean scores for each CEBQ sub-scale were calculated if at least 70% of the items (or 67% in the case of the 3-item ‘Desire to Drink’ sub-scale) were completed, in line with recommendations for dealing with missing data when calculating sub-scale scores (32). Cases with any missing sub-scales were excluded.

To establish that it was appropriate to combine data from the two samples, we tested the sample-by-CEBQ interactions for each sub-scale adjusting for age, sex, ethnicity and parent education in regression analyses on BMI SD-score. When significant, we inspected regression coefficients, confidence intervals and mean scores to check the direction and pattern of the association between CEBQ sub-scale scores and BMI SD in each of the two samples separately. Samples were combined if the patterning of these figures were comparable. For each sub-scale, trend analysis was performed across the five weight groups in order to examine whether adiposity showed a graded association with eating behaviours. When the weighted linear term was significant, each sub-scale was entered into separate regression models, adjusted for age, sex, ethnicity, parent education and sample to determine the proportion of variance explained in BMI SD-score. All analyses were carried out using SPSS v14.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL).

Statement of Ethics

Full ethical approval was gained from University College London Committee on the Ethics of Non-NHS Human Research. All applicable institutional and governmental regulations concerning the ethical use of human volunteers were followed in both studies.


Results
Sample characteristics

Complete data were available for 406 children (Sample 1: n=239; Sample 2: n=167) and they are described in Table 1. In Sample 1, 49% were girls (n=117). Based on IOTF cut-offs, 13.0% of the children were overweight and 3.3% were obese which is low compared with population data for the UK (Health Survey for England, 2006; available from the UK Data Archive: www.data-archive.ac.uk). BMI SD-scores were significantly higher in boys (0.28 ± 1.28) than girls (−0.12 ± 1.27) (t(238)=2.41, p=.016). There was high ethnic diversity compared with UK population data, with 46% of participating children classified as non-white (Office of National Statistics, 2005, available at: www.ons.gov.uk/). Parent education level was similar to the UK average (National Statistics, 2005: www.dcsf.gov.uk/).

In Sample 2, 60.5% were girls. Based on IOTF cut-offs, 18.6% were overweight and 7.2% were obese, which is again slightly lower than UK population data (HSE, 2006). Differences in BMI SD-scores between boys (0.55±1.2) and girls (0.40±1.2) were not significant. The majority of children were white (92.9%), which is comparable to UK population data (ONS, 2005). Parent education level was slightly lower than the national average (National Statistics, 2005).

There were higher proportions of non-white (x2=68.7, p<.0001) and male participants (x2=4.8, p=.028) and the parents had more years of education (x2=8.7, p=.003) in Sample 1 than Sample 2. Children in sample 2 were significantly older (t=−49.5, p<.0001) and had higher BMI SD-scores (t=−3.0, p=.003) than sample 1.

In the combined sample, less educated parents had children with higher BMI SD-scores (t(629)=3.25, p=.001), and older children had higher BMI SD-scores than younger children (r=.12, p=.001). There was no difference in adiposity (BMI SD-score) between white and non-white children (t(409)=−1.05, p=.29) or between boys and girls (t(735)=.13, p=.90).

CEBQ sub-scales

Sub-scales were normally distributed (skewness and kurtosis between 1 and −1) and had good internal consistency, with Cronbach's alphas over .74.

There was a significant interaction between study sample and Enjoyment of Food for BMI SD-score in the adjusted model (F(1,398)=5.17, p=.02). Enjoyment of Food significantly predicted weight in Sample 2 (B(SE)=.50(.13), p<.0001) but not in Sample 1 (B(SE)=.11 (.11), p=.32), although B values were positive in both samples. A significant interaction with sample was also seen for Food Responsiveness (F(1,398)=7.68, p=.006): effects were significant in both samples, but the association between Food Responsiveness and weight was stronger in Sample 2 (B(SE)=.65(.11), p<.0001) than in Sample 1 (B(SE)=.23(.10), p=.02). Inspection of the mean scores by weight group for both of these sub-scales showed that patterns were comparable, with means increasing across weight groups in both samples. No other interactions were significant, so it was deemed appropriate to combine the samples for further analyses.

A significant sex-by-subscale interaction was seen for Food Fussiness (F(1,398)=10.13, p=.002), where the effect was significant in girls (B(SE)=−.27(.10), p=.008) but not in boys (B(SE)=−.11(.10), p=.27. Food fussiness was therefore stratified by sex for further analyses. All other sex-by-subscale interactions were not significant.

Linear Trend Analysis

CEBQ sub-scale scores are shown by IOTF category in Table 2. There were significant positive linear trends by weight group for Food Responsiveness (p<.001), Emotional Overeating (p<.001), Enjoyment of Food (p<.001) and Desire to Drink (p=.037) (Box 1), and significant negative linear trends for Satiety Responsiveness/Slowness in Eating (p<.001) and Food Fussiness in girls (p=.02) and boys (p=.05). The linear trend was not significant for Emotional Undereating (Box 2).

Regression analysis

Table 3 presents a summary of the regression analyses. The baseline model was significant (p=.001) and explained 4.9% of the variance in BMI SD-score. All sub-scales significantly predicted BMI SD-score, explaining 1.0% to 6.3% of additional variance. For every unit increase in Food Responsiveness, Enjoyment of Food, Emotional Overeating and Desire to Drink, BMI SD increased by 0.39 (p<.0001), 0.25 (p=.003), 0.41 (p<.0001) and 0.16 (p=.04) respectively. Every unit increase in Satiety Responsiveness/Slowness in Eating corresponded to a 0.49 decrease in BMI SD-score (p<.0001). Every unit increase in Food Fussiness corresponded to a 0.27 decrease in girls' BMI SD-score but was not significantly associated with adiposity in boys. Analyses with waist SD-score showed similar results and they are not presented here.


DISCUSSION

An appetitive profile characterised by more responsiveness to and enjoyment of food, more emotional eating, lower responsiveness to internal satiety cues, and lower fussiness, was quantitatively associated with weight in a large sample of 7 to 12-year-old, ethnically-diverse, British children. Similar results were obtained using BMI SD-scores and waist SD-scores as outcomes and all effects were independent of age, ethnicity and parental education.

These findings are consistent with earlier research demonstrating that obese children tend to eat more rapidly (7;8), are less sensitive to satiety cues (4;5), and are more responsive to food cues (10) than healthy-weight children. They are also comparable to results recently reported by Viana and colleagues in 3 to 13-year-old Portuguese children, although theirs was a clinical sample with a very small underweight group and they did not subdivide the healthy-weight group (24). They found that the lean children had the lowest positive responses to food and highest avoidant responses, and we found the same results in this study. These results might equally be construed as showing that certain traits are protective from contemporary obesogenic environments as that the opposite traits are risk factors, lending support to the behavioural susceptibility theory of obesity (27).

Our results add to debate regarding the association between fussy eating and BMI (14-16;33). They suggest that, at least in childhood, fussiness could be protective against overeating by reducing the effective choices for a child because of a greater number of dislikes. Alternatively, fussiness may reflect the reverse of enjoyment, so that if eating per se is not as well liked, then desire to eat may not be enough to overcome fussiness.

As far as we are aware, this is the first study to report an association between wish to drink and adiposity, at least in an environment where many of the available drinks are high in energy. The literature in this area is limited and the mechanism by which desire to drink might be related to weight needs more investigation. At one level, it could simply be an expression of wanting something in the mouth; if such an individual is offered caloric drinks, energy intake would be a side-effect, and energy intake from drinks is notoriously non-compensated (34). Alternatively, it could be a specific liking for higher calorie drinks. One study found that higher-risk children (based on maternal pre-pregnancy weight) consumed more fruit juice and less milk at 3 and 4 years, and more sweetened-drinks at 6 years, than lower-risk children, suggesting that caloric drink consumption is a behavioural phenotype for obesity risk (35). Extension of the Desire to Drink subscale to include specific types of drink is necessary to explore these ideas. Another possibility is that increased thirst is associated with weight as a consequence of snack intake; salt - often present in savoury snacks - increases thirst, and recent research describes an association between salt and total fluid and sweet-drink consumption in 4 to 18-year-olds (36). Assessing associations between thirst and food and drink intake is an interesting line of further study.

Our finding that heavier children show higher emotional eating supports the hypothesis that a hyperphagic response to stress and distress increases the risk of weight gain (37). Intake in emotional states tends to favour sweet or fatty foods, which would augment the impact of energy intake (38;39). Emotional undereating was not related to BMI SD-score in this study, and has only been weakly related to BMI in previous work (24). However, we observed higher emotional undereating in both the heaviest and lightest groups, and further research is needed to determine whether this is a true effect, but it does at least indicate that emotional overeating and undereating are not opposites.

Associations between food responsiveness and enjoyment of food and weight group were stronger in sample 2 (from families with twins). Age differences did not explain the effect, because it persisted after controlling for age. It is possible that these effects are due to residual cultural differences as our sample size did not allow full adjustment of the variety of ethnic groups in sample 1. However, because there was no significant sample interaction with the other eating behaviours, it is possible that the findings are due to sampling error. Future research will be needed to identify environmental factors that might moderate the associations between eating behaviour and weight outcomes.

Although limited to English speakers, this study is strengthened by having an ethnically diverse, community-based sample. Including waist circumference as an alternative measure of adiposity - and finding the same results as with BMI SD score - was also valuable in light of evidence that abdominal fatness is associated with negative health outcomes in children (40-42). These features permit generalisability of results to the wider UK population. The study also has limitations. The cross-sectional design means it is not possible to ascertain whether the eating behaviours we assessed were consequences or determinants of adiposity. However, this issue has been most troublesome in studies comparing clinical samples of obese children with healthy-weight groups, because appetitive differences in the obese might reasonably be thought to be a consequence of the worry, dieting, family arguments, school teasing, etc, which obese children receive. The fact that eating behaviours show significant stability (26), and that very thin children were shown to have lower appetites than thinner healthy-weight children, who in turn had lower appetites than fatter healthy-weight children makes it less likely that changes in eating behaviour are a psychosocial consequence of higher weight. Children in sample 1 will be followed up and so it will be possible to assess whether developmental changes during early adolescence (e.g. pubertal growth) influence appetite.

Another limitation is that the sample was relatively lean in comparison with UK population data, which we assume to be due to self-selection out of the study of families with heavier children. In sample 1, it is more likely that heavier children opt-out because measurement takes place at school, whereas in sample 2, measurements took place at home. This may explain weight differences between the samples. However a smaller variation in weight compared to the general population means associations with eating behaviours are more likely to be underestimated than overestimated.

The evidence that CEBQ scores are markers of risk of weight gain makes it important to understand the determinants of these behaviours. Recent work points to a genetic basis (43; 44) with one study demonstrating that genetic influences on weight are partially mediated by satiety (45). As obesity has a strong heritable component, eating behaviours may be one mechanism by which familial risk is transmitted. As a result, food environments in which high energy foods (and drinks) are easily accessible will allow at-risk eating behaviours to manifest. Genetically sensitive, longitudinal designs are necessary to understand the respective influence of genes and environment on the expression of eating behaviours and subsequent weight gain.

Because weight tracks into adulthood, early intervention to prevent expression of ‘at risk’ eating behaviours could be fundamental in curbing further increases in obesity. This study makes it possible to identify healthy-weight individuals who are at risk of weight gain by virtue of their eating behaviour profiles. The challenge then is to discover whether appetitive traits can be modified in ways that reduce obesity risk. Knowing that eating behaviours are quantitatively distributed, it may also be possible to learn more about the behavioural profile that protects children from weight gain and constitutes a ‘lean phenotype’. This could be used as a point of departure from which to develop interventions to modify risky eating behaviours, as well as offering insights into tackling the obesogenic environment.


Acknowledgements

We gratefully acknowledge the ongoing contribution of the parents and children in both the studies. PEACHES is supported by Cancer Research UK (grant GQPM). The Twins Early Development TEDS is supported by a program grant (G0500079) from the U.K. Medical Research Council and the work on obesity in (TEDS) is supported in part by a grant from the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (31/D19086).


Reference List
1. Carnell S,Wardle J. Appetite and adiposity in children: Evidence for a behavioral susceptibility theory of obesityAmer J Clin NutrYear: 200888222918614720
2. Carnell S,Wardle J. Appetitive traits and child obesity: Measurement, origins and implications for interventionProc Nutr SocYear: 2008 In press.
3. Blundell JE,Stubbs RJ,Golding C,Croden F,Alam R,Whybrow S,et al. Resistance and susceptibility to weight gain: Individual variability in response to a high-fat dietPhysiol BehavYear: 20058661462216225895
4. Fisher JO,Cai GW,Jaramillo SJ,Cole SA,Comuzzie AG,Butte NF. Heritability of hyperphagic eating behavior and appetite-related hormones among Hispanic childrenObesityYear: 2007151484149517557986
5. Fisher JO,Birch LL. Eating in the absence of hunger and overweight in girls from 5 to 7 y of ageAm J Clin NutrYear: 20027622623112081839
6. Moens E,Braet C. Predictors of disinhibited eating in children with and without overweightBehav Res TherYear: 2007451357136817101117
7. Barkeling B,Ekman S,Rossner S. Eating behavior in obese and normal weight 11-year-old childrenInt J ObesityYear: 199216355360
8. Drabman RS,Cordua GD,Hammer D,Jarvie GJ,Horton W. Developmental-trends in eating rates of normal and overweight preschool-childrenChild DevYear: 197950211216446206
9. Agras WS,Kraemer HC,Berkowitz RI,Korner AF,Hammer LD. Does a vigorous feeding style influence early development of AdiposityJ PediatricsYear: 1987110799804
10. Jansen A,Theunissen N,Slechten K,Nederkoorn C,Boon B,Mulkens S,Roefs A. Overweight children overeat after exposure to food cuesEating BehaviorsYear: 2003419720915000982
11. Ludwig DS,Peterson KE,Gortmaker SL. Relation between consumption of sugar-sweetened drinks and childhood obesity: a prospective, observational analysisLancetYear: 200135750550811229668
12. Marchi M,Cohen P. Early-childhood eating behaviors and adolescent eating disordersJ Am Acad Child Adolesc PsychiatryYear: 1990291121172295562
13. Galloway AT,Fiorito L,Lee Y,Birch LL. Parental pressure, dietary patterns, and weight status among girls who are “picky eaters”J Am Diet AssocYear: 200510554154815800554
14. Jacobi C,Agras WS,Bryson S,Hammer LD. Behavioral validation, precursors, and concomitants of picky eating in childhoodJ Am Acad Child Adolesc PsychiatryYear: 200342768412500079
15. Carruth BR,Skinner JD. Revisiting the picky eater phenomenon: Neophobic behaviors of young childrenJ Am Coll NutrYear: 20001977178011194531
16. Rydell AM,Dahl M,Sundelin C. Characteristics of school-children who are choosy eatersJ Genet PsycholYear: 19951562172297798080
17. Kaplan HI,Kaplan HS. The psychosomatic concept of obesityJ Nerv Ment DisYear: 195712518120113481715
18. Adam TC,Epel ES. Stress, eating and the reward systemPhysiol BehavYear: 20079144945817543357
19. Caccialanza R,Nicholls D,Cena H,Maccarini L,Rezzani C,Antonioli L,et al. Validation of the Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire parent version (DEBQ-P) in the Italian population: a screening tool to detect differences in eating behaviour among obese, overweight and normal-weight preadolescentsEur J Clin NutrYear: 2004581217122215054434
20. Torres SJ,Nowson CA. Relationship between stress, eating behavior, and obesityNutritionYear: 20072388789417869482
21. Serlachius A,Hamer M,Wardle J. Stress and weight change in university students in the United KingdomPhysiol BehavYear: 20079254855317537466
22. Haworth CMA,Plomin R,Carnell S,Wardle J. Childhood obesity: genetic and environmental overlap with normal-range BMIObesityYear: 2008161585159018421262
23. Wardle J,Guthrie CA,Sanderson S,Rapoport L. Development of the children's eating behaviour questionnaireJ Child Psychol PsycYear: 200142963970
24. Viana V,Sinde S,Saxton JC. Children's Eating Behaviour Questionnaire: Associations with BMI in Portuguese childrenBr J NutrYear: 200810044545018275626
25. Oliver BR,Plomin R. Twins' Early Development Study (TEDS): A multivariate, longitudinal genetic investigation of language, cognition and behavior problems from childhood through adolescenceTwin Res Hum GenetYear: 2007109610517539369
26. Ashcroft J,Semmler C,Carnell S,van Jaarsveld CHM,Wardle J. Continuity and stability of eating behaviour traits in childrenEur J Clin NutrYear: 20086298599017684526
27. Carnell S,Wardle J. Measuring behavioural susceptibility to obesity: Validation of the child eating behaviour questionnaireAppetiteYear: 20074810411316962207
28. Rudolf MCJ,Greenwood DC,Cole TJ,Levine R,Sahota P,Walker J,et al. Rising obesity and expanding waistlines in schoolchildren: A cohort studyArch Dis ChildYear: 20048923523714977700
29. Cole TJ,Freeman JV,Preece MA. Body-Mass Index Reference Curves for the UK, 1990Arch Dis ChildYear: 19957325297639544
30. Cole TJ,Flegal KM,Nicholls D,Jackson AA. Body mass index cut offs to define thinness in children and adolescents: International surveyBMJYear: 200733519417591624
31. Cole TJ,Bellizzi MC,Flegal KM,Dietz WH. Establishing a standard definition for child overweight and obesity worldwide: International surveyBMJYear: 20003201240124310797032
32. Tabachnick BG,Fidell LS. Using Multivariate StatisticsYear: 1996Harper Collins3rd edn.
33. Dovey TM,Staples PA,Gibson EL,Halford JCG. Food neophobia and ‘picky/fussy’ eating in children: A reviewAppetiteYear: 20085018119317997196
34. de Graaf C. Effects of snacks on energy intake: An evolutionary perspectiveAppetiteYear: 200647182316675059
35. Kral TVE,Berkowitz RI,Stunkard AJ,Stallings VA,Faith MS. Caloric beverage consumption in children: A behavioral phenotype for obesity?AppetiteYear: 2008 doi:10.1016/j.appet.2006.03.238. In Press.
36. He FJ,Marrero NM,MacGregor GA. Salt intake is related to soft drink consumption in children and adolescents: A link to obesity?HypertensionYear: 20085162963418287345
37. Braet C,Van Strien T. Assessment of emotional, externally induced and restrained eating behaviour in nine to twelve-year-old obese and non-obese childrenBehavRes TherYear: 199735863873
38. Nguyen-Michel ST,Unger JB,Spruijt-Metz D. Dietary correlates of emotional eating in adolescenceAppetiteYear: 20074949449917466408
39. Oliver G,Wardle J,Gibson EL. Stress and food choice: A laboratory studyPsychosom MedYear: 20006285386511139006
40. McCarthy HD,Ashwell M. A study of central fatness using waist-to-height ratios in UK children and adolescents over two decades supports the simple message - ‘keep your waist circumference to less than half your height’Int J ObesityYear: 200630988992
41. Caprio S,Hyman LD,McCarthy S,Lange R,Bronson M,Tamborlane WV. Fat distribution and cardiovascular risk factors in obese adolescent girls: Importance of intra-abdominal fat depotPediatric ResearchYear: 199639498
42. Freedman DS,Serdula MK,Srinivasan SR,Berenson GS. Relation of circumferences and skinfold thicknesses to lipid and insulin concentrations in children and adolescents: the Bogalusa Heart StudyAm J Clin NutrYear: 1999693083179989697
43. Carnell S,Haworth CM,Plomin R,Wardle J. Genetic influence on appetite in childrenInt J ObesYear: 2008 Epub doi:10.1038/ijo.2008.127.
44. Rankinen T,Bouchard C. Genetics of food intake and eating behavior phenotypes in humansAnnu Rev NutrYear: 20062641343416848714
45. Wardle J,Carnell S,Haworth CMA,Farooqi IS,O'Rahilly S,Plomin R. Obesity-associated genetic variation in FTO is associated with diminished satietyJ Clin Endocrinol MetabYear: 2008 doi:10.1210/jc.2008-0472.

Article Categories:
  • Article

Keywords: BMI, appetite, satiety, hunger, adiposity.

Previous Document:  Gestational weight gain by reduced brain melanocortin activity affects offspring energy balance in r...
Next Document:  Glossopharyngeal pistoning for lung insufflation in patients with cervical spinal cord injury.