Document Detail

Combined pesticide exposure severely affects individual- and colony-level traits in bees.
Jump to Full Text
MedLine Citation:
PMID:  23086150     Owner:  NLM     Status:  MEDLINE    
Abstract/OtherAbstract:
Reported widespread declines of wild and managed insect pollinators have serious consequences for global ecosystem services and agricultural production. Bees contribute approximately 80% of insect pollination, so it is important to understand and mitigate the causes of current declines in bee populations . Recent studies have implicated the role of pesticides in these declines, as exposure to these chemicals has been associated with changes in bee behaviour and reductions in colony queen production. However, the key link between changes in individual behaviour and the consequent impact at the colony level has not been shown. Social bee colonies depend on the collective performance of many individual workers. Thus, although field-level pesticide concentrations can have subtle or sublethal effects at the individual level, it is not known whether bee societies can buffer such effects or whether it results in a severe cumulative effect at the colony level. Furthermore, widespread agricultural intensification means that bees are exposed to numerous pesticides when foraging, yet the possible combinatorial effects of pesticide exposure have rarely been investigated. Here we show that chronic exposure of bumblebees to two pesticides (neonicotinoid and pyrethroid) at concentrations that could approximate field-level exposure impairs natural foraging behaviour and increases worker mortality leading to significant reductions in brood development and colony success. We found that worker foraging performance, particularly pollen collecting efficiency, was significantly reduced with observed knock-on effects for forager recruitment, worker losses and overall worker productivity. Moreover, we provide evidence that combinatorial exposure to pesticides increases the propensity of colonies to fail.
Authors:
Richard J Gill; Oscar Ramos-Rodriguez; Nigel E Raine
Publication Detail:
Type:  Journal Article; Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't     Date:  2012-10-21
Journal Detail:
Title:  Nature     Volume:  491     ISSN:  1476-4687     ISO Abbreviation:  Nature     Publication Date:  2012 Nov 
Date Detail:
Created Date:  2012-11-06     Completed Date:  2012-12-26     Revised Date:  2013-07-11    
Medline Journal Info:
Nlm Unique ID:  0410462     Medline TA:  Nature     Country:  England    
Other Details:
Languages:  eng     Pagination:  105-8     Citation Subset:  IM    
Affiliation:
School of Biological Sciences, Royal Holloway, University of London, Egham, Surrey, TW20 0EX, UK. richard.gill@rhul.ac.uk
Export Citation:
APA/MLA Format     Download EndNote     Download BibTex
MeSH Terms
Descriptor/Qualifier:
Animals
Bees / drug effects*,  physiology*
Behavior, Animal / drug effects*,  physiology
Feeding Behavior / drug effects
Female
Imidazoles / pharmacology
Insecticides / pharmacology*
Male
Nitro Compounds / pharmacology
Pollen / metabolism
Pollination / drug effects
Pyrethrins / pharmacology
Social Behavior*
Social Dominance
Survival Analysis
Grant Support
ID/Acronym/Agency:
094886//Wellcome Trust; //Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council; //Wellcome Trust
Chemical
Reg. No./Substance:
0/Imidazoles; 0/Insecticides; 0/Nitro Compounds; 0/Pyrethrins; 3BN7M937V8/imidacloprid
Comments/Corrections
Comment In:
Nature. 2012 Nov 1;491(7422):43-5   [PMID:  23086148 ]

From MEDLINE®/PubMed®, a database of the U.S. National Library of Medicine

Full Text
Journal Information
Journal ID (nlm-journal-id): 0410462
Journal ID (pubmed-jr-id): 6011
Journal ID (nlm-ta): Nature
Journal ID (iso-abbrev): Nature
ISSN: 0028-0836
ISSN: 1476-4687
Article Information
Download PDF

License:
nihms-submitted publication date: Day: 15 Month: 10 Year: 2012
Electronic publication date: Day: 21 Month: 10 Year: 2012
Print publication date: Day: 1 Month: 11 Year: 2012
pmc-release publication date: Day: 01 Month: 5 Year: 2013
Volume: 491 Issue: 7422
First Page: 105 Last Page: 108
PubMed Id: 23086150
ID: 3495159
DOI: 10.1038/nature11585
ID: EMS49946

Combined pesticide exposure severely affects individual- and colony-level traits in bees
Richard J. Gill
Oscar Ramos-Rodriguez
Nigel E. Raine
School of Biological Sciences, Royal Holloway, University of London, Egham, Surrey, TW20 0EX
Correspondence: Correspondence and requests for materials should be addresses to RJG (richard.gill@rhul.ac.uk) or NER (nigel.raine@rhul.ac.uk).
Author Contributions RJG, ORR & NER carried-out the experiment, RJG & NER designed the experiment and wrote the paper, NER conceived the project.

The majority of studies to date have focused on pesticide exposure in honeybees, but bumblebees are also crucial pollinators and have smaller colonies making them ideally suited to investigate effects at both the individual (worker) and colony level. This study mimicked a realistic scenario in which 40 early stage bumblebee, Bombus terrestris, colonies received long-term (4-week) exposure to two widely used pesticides frequently encountered when foraging on flowering crops, the neonicotinoid Imidacloprid and the pyrethroid λ-cyhalothrin. Imidacloprid is a systemic pesticide found in all plant tissues including the pollen and nectar consumed by bees (oral exposure18-20). In contrast, λ-cyhalothrin is sprayed directly on to crops, including their flowers, to which bees will be topically exposed (details in Supplementary Information). Foraging bees are thus simultaneously exposed to both chemicals in the field, making them excellent candidates to investigate the potential for combinatorial effects of pesticide exposure. Using a split block design (see Methods), we monitored colonies exposed to each pesticide independently and in combination (ten Control colonies: ten exposed to Imidacloprid (I), ten exposed to λ-cyhalothrin (LC) and ten exposed to I and LC (Mix = M)). Imidacloprid (dissolved in 40% sucrose solution) was provided at a concentration (10 ppb) within the range found in crop nectar and pollen in the field9,21. λ-cyhalothrin was administered following label guidance for field spray application (see Supplementary Information). Bees were able to forage in the field providing a realistic and demanding behavioural setting, and the foraging behaviour of individual workers was recorded using radio frequency identification (RFID) tagging technology10,11,22 (Supplementary Figs 1 and 2). Colonies were motivated to forage because we provided them with no pollen and limited amounts of sucrose solution.

During colony development the production of workers (and their survival) is vital to colony success because workers provide the labour (e.g. brood care, foraging) for the colony. Total worker production at the end of the experiment was significantly lower in Imidacloprid treated colonies (reduced by 27% in I and 9% in M colonies) compared to Control colonies (mean (±s.e.m.) workers per colony: I=19.7±3.0, M=24.4±3.2 vs Control=27.0±4.0; Linear Mixed Effects model (LMER): I: Z=−3.71, P<0.001; M: Z=−2.62, P=0.009; Fig. 1A). Two of the 40 colonies, both M colonies, did not survive the experiment (they ‘failed’ after 3 & 8 days; see Supplementary Information), a colony failure rate significantly higher than other treatments (Fisher’s Exact test: mid-p correction=0.029). These two colonies were excluded from the analyses to provide a conservative assessment of worker production in M colonies (when included in analysis=20.0±3.9 workers). During the experiment 223 (21% of total) workers were found dead inside nest-boxes. On average, 36±7.3% and 39±7.5% of workers from LC and M colonies respectively died in the nest-box: a figure four times higher than Control (9±3.4%) colonies (LMER): LC: t=4.31, P<0.001; M: t=4.23, P<0.001; Fig. 1B). Moreover, 43% of the workers found dead in LC and M colonies lived fewer than four days after eclosion – an apparent waste of resources required for future colony growth given that such young members are unlikely to have contributed any work (e.g. foraging) to offset the resources invested to produce them. Queen loss occurred in 14 colonies, although loss rate did not differ significantly among treatments (Control=4; I=5; LC=2; M=3; Fisher’s exact test: mid-P-correction=0.40) and we accounted for queen loss in our analyses (see Supplementary Information).

Daily counts of newly eclosed bees showed worker production in I colonies did not become significantly lower than Control colonies until the end of week-2, and for M colonies until the end of week-4 (Fig. 1C; see Supplementary Information and Supplementary Table 1). Daily counts of dead bees also revealed that worker mortality in LC colonies did not become significantly higher than Control colonies until the end of week-3, but for M colonies as early as the end of week-1. The delayed effect of Imidacloprid exposure on worker productivity in I and M colonies coincides with the time taken by workers to develop from egg to adult (~22 days) suggesting the observed effect is a result of Imidacloprid on brood development. Indeed, the total number of larvae and pupae combined found in colonies at the end of the experiment (‘brood number’) was significantly lower in I and M colonies compared to Control colonies (LMER: I: Z=−6.23, P<0.001; M: Z=−5.60, P<0.001). Overall, this represented a 22% reduction in brood production in I colonies and a 7% reduction in M colonies (mean (±s.e.m.) brood number: I=36±8.0, M=43±11.7 [including failed colonies: M=39±9.6] vs Control=46±9.7). Despite this, there was no significant difference in the mass of nest-structure across treatments at the end of the experiment (LMER: I: t=−1.12, P=0.27; M: t=−1.22, P=0.23; Supplementary Fig. 3) indicating that I and M colonies attempted to raise similar brood numbers but a lower proportion of larvae/pupae survived to eclosion.

Whilst Imidacloprid could be directly affecting brood (physiological) development, it could also indirectly affect the brood by causing changes to colony behaviour and/or structure: for example, changes to foraging behaviour leading to food limitation23,24. We tested the latter hypothesis by studying worker foraging performance using RFID technology to automatically record the exact time workers left or entered each colony (Supplementary Figs 1 and 2). Overall, we collected data from 259 recognised foragers from 32 colonies (n colonies: Control=7; I=10; LC=8; M=7) making 8751 foraging bouts (median (inter-quartile range) per worker=23(10-44); for criteria used to classify foragers and foraging bouts see Methods). We examined whether pesticide treatment affected foraging activity and forager recruitment. We found that foragers from M colonies performed fewer foraging bouts compared to Control colonies (LMER: t=−2.55, P=0.011; Fig. 2A), and that there were significantly more foragers in both I and M colonies compared to Control colonies over the 4-weeks (LMER: I: Z=4.20, P<0.001; M: Z=3.49, P<0.001; Fig. 2A). The higher number of foragers in I and M colonies (compared to Control) is unlikely to be due to either pesticide causing a significant repellent or anti-feedant effect (this corroborates the lack of published evidence for pyrethroid repellency in bumblebees despite reports of pyrethroids being repellent to honeybees25). Although workers did not have to visit the feeder, as they could forage for nectar outside, we found no difference among treatments in the amount of sucrose collected from feeders (LMER: t≤1.63, P≥0.11; Supplementary Fig. 6).

Given that I and M colonies recruited higher numbers of workers to forage we evaluated whether this was a response to reduced individual foraging efficiency by monitoring pollen foraging performance and observing the size of pollen loads (load size scored as: small=1, medium=2, large=3; see Methods) brought back by foragers (n=20 hours of observation per colony). Crucially, Imidacloprid exposed foragers returned with significantly smaller pollen loads per foraging bout compared to Control colonies (LMER: I: t=−3.31, P=0.0011; M: t=−3.38, P<0.001; Fig. 2B). Imidacloprid exposed foragers collected pollen successfully in a significantly lower percentage of their foraging bouts (mean (±s.e.m.): I=59±7.3%, M=55±8.6% vs Control=82±5.8%; LMER: I: t=−3.16, P=0.0018; M: t=−3.05, P=0.0026; Supplementary Fig. 4) and we also found that the average duration of successful foraging bouts (during which pollen was collected) was significantly longer for Imidacloprid exposed foragers than for Control foragers (LMER: I: t=2.10, P=0.037; M: t=2.87, P=0.005; Fig. 2C). Together, these data show that Imidacloprid exposed workers were significantly less efficient at collecting pollen in the field.

A consequence of recruiting a greater number of workers to forage is that it increases the proportion of colony workforce going outside to undertake a potentially hazardous task22. Indeed, our RFID data show the number of foragers per colony was significantly correlated with the number of workers leaving the colony and getting ‘lost’ outside (i.e. workers that did not return: Spearman’s Rank: ρ=0.801, P<0.001; Supplementary Fig. 5). Consequently, we found that on average the percentage of workers getting lost in I and M colonies was 50% and 55% higher than Control colonies (I=30±3.1%, M=31±5.3% vs Control=20±2.9%; LMER: I: t=2.83, P=0.008; M: t=2.26, P=0.03). Furthermore, when considering worker mortality and losses combined over the 4-weeks (mean (±s.e.m): I=41±4.2%, LC=51±6.8%, M=69±7.1% vs Control=30±5.0%, LMER: I: t=1.79, P=0.08; LC: t=3.25, P=0.0026; M: t=5.24, P<0.001; Table 1, Fig. 3), we found that colonies treated with both pesticides (M) suffered most severely. Moreover, M colonies had significantly higher overall worker losses than either I colonies (LMER: t=−3.69, P<0.001) or LC colonies (LMER: t=−2.31, P=0.027).

We have shown that Imidacloprid exposure at concentrations that can be found in the pollen and nectar of flowering crops causes impairment to pollen foraging efficiency, leading to increased colony demand for food as shown by increased worker recruitment to forage. However, Imidacloprid treated colonies (I and M) were still unable to collect as much pollen as Control colonies. Such pollen constraints, coupled with a higher number of workers undertaking foraging rather than brood care, appeared to affect brood development resulting in reduced worker production which can only exacerbate the problem of having an impaired colony workforce. These findings show a mechanistic explanation to link recently reported effects on individual worker behaviour10,11,26-29 and colony queen production12 as a result of neonicotinoid exposure. Moreover, exposure to a second pesticide λ-cyhalothrin (pyrethroid) applied at label guideline concentration for crop use caused additional worker mortality in this study highlighting another potential risk. Bee colonies typically encounter multiple classes of pesticides when foraging in the field13-15 potentially exposing them to a range of combinatorial effects. Indeed, M colonies in our study were consistently negatively affected in all our measures of worker behaviour, suffered the highest overall worker losses (i.e. worker mortality and forager losses) which were twice as great as Control colonies, and two colonies did in fact fail (Table 1).

Pesticide label guidance concentrations and application rates are approved on the basis of ecotoxicological tests using single pesticides and set at a level for field use deemed ‘sublethal’ (i.e. below LD50). However, the risk of exposure to multiple pesticides, or from the same pesticide being applied to different (adjacent) crops, is currently not considered when evaluating the safety of pesticides for (honey) bees. Given the serious impacts on M colonies it is concerning that pesticide products containing mixtures of neonicotinoids and pyrethroids are in current use18. At present there are also no guidelines for testing chronic or sublethal effects of pesticides on bees30, and considering we did not detect significant effects until 2-4 weeks into our study, the current EPPO/OECD guideline of 96-hours (max) exposure (for testing acute effects of pesticides on honeybees) appears insufficient. Our results emphasise the importance of recent recommendations by the EFSA Panel on Plant Protection Products and their Residues (http://www.efsa.europa.eu/en/efsajournal/pub/2668.htm) proposing the need for longer term toxicity testing on both adult bees and larvae, new protocols to detect cumulative toxicity effects, and separate risk assessment schemes for different bee species. Our findings have clear implications for the conservation of insect pollinators in areas of agricultural intensification, especially social bees with their complex social organisation and dependence on a critical threshold of workers performing efficiently to ensure colony survival.


METHODS
Experimental setup

Each colony contained a queen and an average of four workers (range=0-10), at the start of the experiment, reflecting the development stage of natural colonies when crops tend to flower in Europe31,32, and when most pesticide treatments are applied (March-June)33,34. We used a split block design to account for variation in colony size, developmental stage and potential seasonal variation between replicates (20 colonies in July, and 20 colonies in September: see Supplementary Information). For each replicate, colonies were ranked according to the number of workers and pupae with the four highest ranked (largest) colonies assigned to block-1, the next four highest ranked to block-2, etc. Each replicate consisted of five blocks (n=20 colonies). Within each block the four treatments (Control, I, LC, M) were randomly assigned among the four colonies. There was no significant difference among treatments in either the number of workers or pupae present at the start of the experiment (Supplementary Information). Colonies were provided a two-chambered nest-box; the rear chamber housing the nest (‘brood chamber’) and front chamber used for pesticide exposure (‘food chamber’: Supplementary Figs. 1 & 6). Nest-boxes were kept in the laboratory but connected to the outside environment via an outlet tube leading to an exit hole in the laboratory window, allowing natural foraging (for details see Supplementary Information and Supplementary Fig. 1). Between the outlet tube and nest-box were three sections of transparent tubing allowing observation of bees as they left or entered nest-boxes (Supplementary Fig. 2). Two RFID readers (Maja IV reader modules with optimized antenna for mic3 transponders: Microsensys GmbH) at the nest entrance allowed automatic monitoring of all tagged workers as they entered and left the colony with minimal disturbance to natural foraging patterns22.

Pesticide treatment

Bees were exposed to pesticide treatments in the food chamber using a gravity feeder placed on a petri-dish (90mm diameter) lined with filter paper. The filter paper was sprayed with 0.69±0.046ml of either control solution (Control & I), or 37.5ppm λ-cyhalothrin solution (LC & M) – the maximum label guidance concentration for spray application to oilseed rape in the UK. The gravity feeder contained either a control/sucrose solution (Control & LC) or 10ppb Imidacloprid/sucrose solution (I & M). This concentration falls within the range found in the pollen and nectar of flowering crops visited by bees9,20,21,35-38 (for details on pesticide selection and application see Supplementary Information and Supplementary Box 1). During the experiment the sucrose treatment was applied every two days [three days over weekends] between 13.00-14.00h (Supplementary Table 2). Before refilling feeders we measured the volume of any remaining solution to calculate what the bees had collected (n = 12 feeder replenishments per colony during the 28-day period). We provided 10ml of sucrose treatment per application in week-1, with a 2ml increment at the start of each subsequent week (i.e. week-2 = 12ml, week-3 = 14ml, week-4 = 16ml) to reflect an increase in colony demand as they developed. The amount of sugar provided was less than each colony typically collects by nectar foraging39 ensuring that workers were motivated to forage for nectar and pollen outside.

Spray treatments were applied once at the start of each experimental week (Supplementary Table 2) using a new piece of filter paper for each application. This follows label guidance for maximum application of λ-cyhalothrin to crops recommending at least seven days between spraying events and up to four applications within the flowering season.

Observations and measurements
i) Colony condition and development

Colonies were inspected everyday to assess the number of newly eclosed (callow) workers, the number of dead workers (removed and frozen (−20°C)), and queen condition. Three days before the start of the experiment faecal samples from each queen were checked for the presence of three parasites: the trypanosome Crithidia bombi, the microsporidian Nosema bombi, and the neogregarine Apicystis bombi. This parasite assessment was repeated on the 28th experimental day using faecal samples from the queen (if present) and a subset of workers from each nest-box (for details of parasite assessment see Supplementary Information).

ii) Monitoring foraging performance

All workers present at the start of the experiment (precise age unknown) were individually RFID tagged (for details see Supplementary Information), and during the experiment all newly produced workers were tagged within three days of eclosion (age known). Tagging stopped on the 24th day of the experiment because any workers emerging after this point were unlikely to become foragers40. In total 854 workers were tagged, with each tag providing a unique (16 digit) code for unambiguous identification. We classified a foraging bout as a period of at least five minutes elapsing between a worker leaving and entering a colony. We also specified that workers must perform at least four foraging bouts to be considered a forager (for rationale behind foraging rules see Supplementary Information).

Pollen foraging was observed in each colony for one hour per day (five days a week) to record pollen foraging activity. Observation periods were always two (approx. 16.00) and 21 hours (approx. 10.00 the following day) after treatment application/renewal (Supplementary Table 2). We recorded the time that each tagged worker entered a colony (observing when it passed through the transparent tubes and under the RFID readers) using a stopwatch synchronised with the RFID (host) data-logger. We scored the amount of pollen in the forager’s corbiculae (pollen baskets) as small (score of 1), medium (score of 2) or large (score of 3) relative to the size of the worker.

iii) End of experiment

Nest-box entrances were closed after dark on the evening of the 28th experimental day. Each nest-box, containing bees and brood, was placed in a freezer (−20°C). Window exits remained open for 18 hours with each outlet tube connected to an individual bottle trap to catch any returning foragers. All tagged workers were identified and recently eclosed (untagged) workers assumed to have developed in the colony they were found in. Worker thorax width was measured using digital callipers. All pupae and larvae were dissected from each nest, counted and weighed to provide final measures of brood development, and the nest-structure was also weighed.



Notes

FN1Supplementary Information is linked to the online version of the paper at www.nature.com/nature.

FN3Author Information The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Acknowledgements

We thank Mark Brown, Mark Clook, Jim Culverhouse, Adrian Dixon, Matthias Fürst, Dave Garthwaite, Alice Horsell, Vincent Jansen and Inti Pedroso-Rovira for comments and technical assistance, and Syngenta Bioline Bees for supplying colonies. The study was supported by the Insect Pollinator Initiative (funded under auspices of the Living with Environmental Change programme, BBSRC, Wellcome Trust, Scottish Government, DEFRA and NERC: BB/I000178/1).


References
1. Biesmeijer JC,et al. Parallel declines in pollinators and insect-pollinated plants in Britain and the NetherlandsScienceYear: 200631335135416857940
2. Klein AM,et al. Importance of pollinators in changing landscapes for world cropsProc. R. Soc. Lond. BYear: 2007274303313
3. Kremen C,et al. Pollination and other ecosystem services produced by mobile organisms: a conceptual framework for the effects of land-use changeEcol. LettYear: 20071029931417355569
4. Potts SG,et al. Global pollinator declines: trends, impacts and driversTrends Ecol. EvolYear: 20102534535320188434
5. Oldroyd BP. What’s killing American honey bees?PLoS BiolYear: 20075e16817564497
6. Brown MJF,Paxton RJ. The conservation of bees: a global perspectiveApidologieYear: 200940410416
7. Thompson HM. Behavioural effects of pesticides in bees - their potential for use in risk assessmentEcotoxicologyYear: 20031231733012739878
8. Desneux N,Decourtye A,Delpuech JM. The sublethal effects of pesticides on beneficial arthropodsAnnu. Rev. EntomolYear: 2007528110616842032
9. Cresswell JE. A meta-analysis of experiments testing the effects of a neonicotinoid insecticide (Imidacloprid) on honey beesEcotoxicologyYear: 20112014915721080222
10. Schneider CW,Tautz J,Grünewald B,Fuchs S. RFID tracking of sublethal effects of two neonicotinoid insecticides on the foraging behavior of Apis melliferaPLoS OneYear: 20127e3002322253863
11. Henry M,et al. A common pesticide decreases foraging success and survival in honey beesScienceYear: 201233634835022461498
12. Whitehorn PR,O’Connor S,Wackers FL,Goulson D. Neonicotinoid pesticide reduces bumble bee colony growth and queen productionScienceYear: 201233635135222461500
13. Johnson RM,Ellis MD,Mullin CA,Frazier M. Pesticides and honey bee toxicity - USAApidologieYear: 201041312331
14. Mullin CA,et al. High levels of miticides and agrochemicals in North American apiaries: implications for honey bee healthPLoS OneYear: 20105e975420333298
15. Krupke CH,Hunt GJ,Eitzer BD,Andino G,Given K. Multiple routes of pesticide exposure for honey bees living near agricultural fieldsPLoS OneYear: 20127e2926822235278
16. Johnson RM,Pollock HS,Berenbaum MR. Synergistic interactions between in-hive miticides in Apis melliferaJ. Econ. EntomolYear: 200910247447919449624
17. Pilling ED,Jepson PC. Synergism between EBI fungicides and a pyrethroid insecticide in the honeybee (Apis mellifera)Pestic. SciYear: 199339293297
18. Elbert A,Haas M,Springer B,Thielert W,Nauen R. Applied aspects of neonicotinoid uses in crop protectionPest Manag. SciYear: 2008641099110518561166
19. Rortais A,Arnold G,Halm MP,Touffet-Briens F. Modes of honeybees exposure to systemic insecticides: estimated amounts of contaminated pollen and nectar consumed by different categories of beesApidologieYear: 2005367183
20. Chauzat MP,et al. A survey of pesticide residues in pollen loads collected by honey bees in FranceJ. Econ. EntomolYear: 20069925326216686121
21. Blacquière T,Smagghe G,van Gestel CAM,Mommaerts V. Neonicotinoids in bees: a review on concentrations, side-effects and risk assessmentEcotoxicologyYear: 20122197399222350105
22. Molet M,Chittka L,Stelzer RJ,Streit S,Raine NE. Colony nutritional status modulates worker responses to foraging recruitment pheromone in the bumblebee Bombus terrestrisBehav. Ecol. SociobiolYear: 20086219191926
23. Schmid-Hempel R,Schmid-Hempel P. Colony performance and immunocompetence of a social insect, Bombus terrestris, in poor and variable environmentsFunct. EcolYear: 1998122230
24. Pelletier L,McNeil JN. The effect of food supplementation on reproductive success in bumblebee field coloniesOikosYear: 2003103688694
25. Thompson H,Wilkins S. Assessment of the synergy and repellency of pyrethroid/fungicide mixturesB. InsectolYear: 200356131134
26. Bortolotti L,et al. Effects of sub-lethal imidacloprid doses on the homing rate and foraging activity of honey beesB. InsectolYear: 2003566367
27. Decourtye A,Devillers J,Cluzeau S,Charreton M,Pham-Delègue MH. Effects of imidacloprid and deltamethrin on associative learning in honeybees under semi-field and laboratory conditionsEcotox. Environ. SafeYear: 200457410419
28. Mommaerts V,et al. Risk assessment for side-effects of neonicotinoids against bumblebees with and without impairing foraging behaviorEcotoxicologyYear: 20101920721519757031
29. Yang EC,Chuang YC,Chen YL,Chang LH. Abnormal foraging behavior induced by sublethal dosage of Imidacloprid in the Honey bee (Hymenoptera: Apidae)J. Econ. EntomolYear: 20081011743174819133451
30. Halm MP,Rortais A,Arnold G,Taséi JN,Rault S. New risk assessment approach for systemic insecticides: the case of honey bees and imidacloprid (Gaucho)Environ. Sci. TechnolYear: 2006402448245416646488

References for Methods section
31. Thompson HM. Assessing the exposure and toxicity of pesticides to bumblebees (Bombus sp.)ApidologieYear: 200132305321
32. Brittain C,Potts SG. The potential impacts of insecticides on the life-history traits of bees and the consequences for pollinationBasic Appl. EcolYear: 201112321331
33. Garthwaite D,Thomas MR,Parrish G,Smith L,Barker I. Pesticide Usage Survey Report 224. Arable crops in Great Britain 2008 (including aerial applications 07-08)Year: 2008Food and Environmental Research AgencyYork, UK
34. Garthwaite D,et al. Pesticide Usage Survey Report 235. Arable crops in UK 2010 (including aerial applications 2010)Year: 2010Food and Environmental Research AgencyYork, UK
35. Bonmatin JM,et al. A LC/APCI-MS/MS method for analysis of imidacloprid in soils, in plants, and in pollensAnal. ChemYear: 2003752027203312720336
36. Bonmatin JM,et al. Quantification of imidacloprid uptake in maize cropsJ. Agric. Food ChemYear: 2005535336534115969515
37. Chauzat MP,et al. Influence of pesticide residues on honey bee (Hymenoptera: Apidae) colony health in FranceEnviron. EntomolYear: 20093851452319508759
38. Krischik VA,Landmark AL,Heimpel GE. Soil-applied imidacloprid is translocated to nectar and kills nectar-feeding Anagyrus pseudococci (Girault) (Hymenoptera: Encyrtidae)Environ. EntomolYear: 2007361238124518284749
39. Raine NE,Chittka L. The correlation of learning speed and natural foraging success in bumble-beesProc. R. Soc. Lond. BYear: 2008275803808
40. Goulson D. Bumblebees: Behaviour, Ecology and ConservationYear: 2010Oxford Univ. Press

Article Categories:
  • Article


Previous Document:  Structure and mechanism of a bacterial sodium-dependent dicarboxylate transporter.
Next Document:  [World Health Organization document "Water safety in buildings": Italian translation].