Document Detail

Branding and a child's brain: an fMRI study of neural responses to logos.
Jump to Full Text
MedLine Citation:
PMID:  22997054     Owner:  NLM     Status:  Publisher    
Abstract/OtherAbstract:
Branding and advertising have a powerful effect on both familiarity and preference for products, yet no neuroimaging studies have examined neural response to logos in children. Food advertising is particularly pervasive and effective in manipulating choices in children. The purpose of the present study was to examine how healthy children's brains respond to common food and other logos. A pilot validation study was first conducted with 32 children to select the most culturally familiar logos, and to match food and nonfood logos on valence and intensity. A new sample of 17 healthy weight children were then scanned using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Food logos compared to baseline were associated with increased activation in orbitofrontal cortex and inferior prefrontal cortex. Compared to nonfood logos, food logos elicited increased activation in posterior cingulate cortex. Results confirmed that food logos activate some brain regions in children known to be associated with motivation. This marks the first study in children to examine brain responses to culturally familiar logos. Considering the pervasiveness of advertising, research should further investigate how children respond at the neural level to marketing.
Authors:
Amanda S Bruce; Jared M Bruce; William R Black; Rebecca J Lepping; Janice M Henry; J Bradley C Cherry; Laura E Martin; Vlad B Papa; Ann M Davis; William M Brooks; Cary R Savage
Related Documents :
22014724 - Food sources and correlates of sodium and potassium intakes in flemish pre-school child...
16166564 - Fast food, central nervous system insulin resistance, and obesity.
19826184 - Magnetically induced vagus nerve stimulation and feeding behavior in rats.
11011954 - Aetiology of overweight and obesity in children and adolescents.
12954394 - Melatonin and methimazole mimic short-day-induced fattening in gray mouse lemurs.
12828194 - Carbohydrate, memory, and mood.
Publication Detail:
Type:  JOURNAL ARTICLE     Date:  2012-9-20
Journal Detail:
Title:  Social cognitive and affective neuroscience     Volume:  -     ISSN:  1749-5024     ISO Abbreviation:  Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci     Publication Date:  2012 Sep 
Date Detail:
Created Date:  2012-9-21     Completed Date:  -     Revised Date:  -    
Medline Journal Info:
Nlm Unique ID:  101288795     Medline TA:  Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci     Country:  -    
Other Details:
Languages:  ENG     Pagination:  -     Citation Subset:  -    
Affiliation:
MO 64110 bruceam@umkc.edu.
Export Citation:
APA/MLA Format     Download EndNote     Download BibTex
MeSH Terms
Descriptor/Qualifier:

From MEDLINE®/PubMed®, a database of the U.S. National Library of Medicine

Full Text
Journal Information
Journal ID (nlm-ta): Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci
Journal ID (iso-abbrev): Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci
Journal ID (publisher-id): scan
Journal ID (hwp): scan
ISSN: 1749-5016
ISSN: 1749-5024
Publisher: Oxford University Press
Article Information
Download PDF
© The Author (2012). Published by Oxford University Press
creative-commons:
Received Day: 30 Month: 5 Year: 2012
Accepted Day: 7 Month: 9 Year: 2012
Print publication date: Month: 1 Year: 2014
Electronic publication date: Day: 14 Month: 12 Year: 2012
pmc-release publication date: Day: 14 Month: 12 Year: 2012
Volume: 9 Issue: 1
First Page: 118 Last Page: 122
PubMed Id: 22997054
ID: 3871732
DOI: 10.1093/scan/nss109
Publisher Id: nss109

Branding and a child’s brain: an fMRI study of neural responses to logos
Amanda S. Bruce12
Jared M. Bruce1
William R. Black1
Rebecca J. Lepping34
Janice M. Henry1
Joseph Bradley C. Cherry1
Laura E. Martin56
Vlad B. Papa5
Ann M. Davis27
William M. Brooks58
Cary R. Savage39
1Department of Psychology, University of Missouri-Kansas City, 5030 Cherry Street, Kansas City, MO 64110, MO, USA, 2Center for Children’s Healthy Lifestyles and Nutrition, University of Kansas Medical Center/Children’s Mercy Hospital, 610 E. 22nd Street, Kansas City, MO 64108, KS, USA, 3Center for Health Behavior Neuroscience, University of Kansas Medical Center, 3901 Rainbow Boulevard, Kansas City, KS 66160, KS, USA, 4Department of Psychology, University of Kansas, 1415 Jayhawk Blvd., Fraser Hall, Room 426, Lawrence, KS 66045-7556, KS, USA, 5Hoglund Brain Imaging Center, University of Kansas Medical Center, 3901 Rainbow Boulevard, Kansas City, KS 66160, KS, USA, 6Department of Preventive Medicine, University of Kansas Medical Center, 3901 Rainbow Boulevard, Kansas City, KS 66160, KS, USA, 7Department of Pediatrics, University of Kansas Medical Center, 3901 Rainbow Boulevard, Kansas City, KS 66160, KS, USA, 8Department of Neurology, University of Kansas Medical Center, 3901 Rainbow Boulevard, Kansas City, KS 66160, KS, USA and 9Department of Psychiatry, University of Kansas Medical Center, 3901 Rainbow Boulevard, Kansas City, KS 66160, KS, USA
Correspondence: Correspondence should be addressed to Amanda S. Bruce, PhD, Assistant Professor, Department of Psychology, The University of Missouri-Kansas City, 5030 Cherry Street, 307 Cherry Hall, Kansas City, MO 64110, USA. E-mail: bruceam@umkc.edu

INTRODUCTION

Advertising is a dominant industry in the United States with food and beverage companies alone spending more than $10 billion annually to market their products to children (Institute of Medicine Committee, 2006). The intense marketing toward youth is driven by companies’ ambitions for brand recognition, preference and loyalty. The average child in the United States views more than 5500 television food advertisements per year (Federal Trade Commission, 2007). Of these, 98% are for products high in fat, sugar and/or sodium (Powell et al., 2007). Advertising is successful with studies on the effects of television food advertising showing that children exposed to advertisements will prefer advertised foods at much higher rates than children who were not exposed (Coon and Tucker, 2002). Also, the amount of exposure children have to food advertisements directly impacts the number of attempts they make to influence their parents’ purchases (Coon and Tucker, 2002). Robinson et al. (2007) asked children aged 3–5 years to taste identical foods and beverages labeled in McDonald’s™ or unbranded packaging. Although the food and drink samples were identical, children indicated a statistically significant preference for the taste of food and drinks labeled with McDonald’s™ brand logos, exemplifying how food advertising impacts children’s preferences and food motivation. The consensus among published reviews is that ‘food promotion has a causal and direct effect on children’s food preferences, knowledge, and behavior’ (Livingstone, 2005: 283). In addition, some experts have cited food marketing as one of the contributors to the recent rise in childhood obesity (Harris et al., 2009).

Neuroimaging techniques such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) can help to improve understanding of how people process, evaluate and respond to product brands (see Plassman et al., 2012 for a review). Published neuromarketing studies of healthy adults viewing culturally familiar logos have determined that the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and hippocampus are involved in brand recognition. Specifically, product brands activate dorsolateral PFC, ventromedial PFC, orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), ventral striatum and hippocampus (e.g. McClure et al., 2004; Schaefer et al., 2006; Schaefer and Rotte, 2007a,b; Schaefer and Rotte, 2011; Esch et al., 2012). Moreover, the PFC, OFC, ACC, ventral striatum and hippocampus have also been identified as being involved in food motivation, reward processing and general appetitive cues (as both ‘drive’ and ‘control’ regions) (e.g. Gautier et al., 2000; Small et al., 2001; DelParigi et al., 2005; Simmons et al., 2005; Martin et al., 2010).

Studies on children’s brain responses to actual food images have implicated similar brain regions as those identified in adults (Holsen et al., 2005; Killgore & Yurgelun-Todd, 2005; Bruce et al., 2010; Davids et al., 2010). In healthy weight children, one fMRI study compared brain activations in response to appetizing food images when children were hungry and when they were satiated (Holsen et al., 2005). Increased activations to food images were reported in insula, amygdala, medial frontal cortex and OFC, which are similar to adult findings. Another study compared adolescent and adult brain activation and identified increased activation to food images in OFC and hippocampus (Killgore & Yurgelun-Todd, 2005). The neural networks associated with food motivation are the same regions discussed in the well-supported theory of brain development (Casey et al., 2000). This theoretical model posits that the increase in risk-taking behavior in adolescence is attributed to uneven neurobiological development in brain regions associated with cognitive control and emotional drive (Somerville and Casey, 2010). Specifically, reward regions including striatum mature before the cognitive and self-control regions of the PFC. Therefore, without the necessary inhibitory processes to aid in decision making, youth are particularly susceptible to making poor health behavior choices and these differences may be particularly pronounced when evaluating appetitive cues (Somerville and Casey, 2010).

Despite recent interest in neuromarketing and the neuroscience of food motivation, no studies thus far have examined brain activation in children viewing brand logos. Therefore, the aim of this study was to examine neural responses to product brands in children to gain a better understanding of how children’s brains respond to appetitive cues frequently used in advertising. We hypothesized an increase in activity in the limbic and paralimbic system, including ventral striatum, and prefrontal brain regions when children were viewing food logos compared with either non-food logos or a baseline condition. We used an fMRI stimulus paradigm including familiar food and non-food logos that were common in the United States, e.g. McDonald’s arches®, Lucky Charms™ leprechaun, Rice Krispies™ elves vs the Target™ bulls-eye, the Energizer Bunny®, FedEx® vs blurred images of logos matched on color composition and brightness (baseline condition) as comparisons. A better understanding of children’s responses to food logos will be beneficial in elucidating the complex relationships between advertising and neural responses to motivational cues.


MATERIALS AND METHODS

The protocols for the pilot validation study and the main fMRI study were approved by the Human Subjects Committee at the University of Kansas Medical Center (KUMC). Written informed consent was obtained from each child’s parent/legal guardian and written informed assent was obtained from each child before study participation.

Validation of logo stimuli

A validation study was first conducted to select the most appropriate logos for use in the activation paradigm. Thirty-two participants (13 males) aged 9–16 years (mean = 11.5 years, s.d. = 2.2) rated 239 culturally familiar brand logos on a five-point Likert scale on three categories: familiarity, valence (happy/sad) and arousal (exciting/boring) (Figure 1). This standardized scale has been used in many stimulus validation studies for the International Affective Picture Set (Lang et al., 2008). Based on the participants’ ratings, 60 food and 60 non-food logos that were high on familiarity were selected (see online Supplementary Data). Food logos as a group were matched on familiarity with non-food logos [t(118) = 0.33, P = 0.74]. The food and non-food logos were not significantly different on valence [t(118) = 1.26, P= 0.21] or arousal [t(118) = 1.49, P = 0.14]. These 120 logos were used in the fMRI paradigm in the main study described later. Baseline images were created from the food and non-food logos using three iterations of a fast Fourier transform to render the logos unidentifiable. The baseline images were therefore matched to the food and non-food logos on visual properties of color composition and brightness.

Main fMRI study
Participants

Seventeen children (10 males) with a mean age of 11.8 years (s.d. = 1.4, range 10–14) were recruited from broadcast email messages sent to the KUMC employees and from the pediatric clinic. All participants were in age-appropriate grades. Exclusion criteria included major psychiatric diagnoses and neurological illness (parental interview), left-handedness and impaired, uncorrected vision. All participants spoke English as their primary language. None of these participants took part in the validation study.

Procedures and methods

After informed consent was obtained, participants and their parents completed demographic measures. Time since last food intake was at least 4 h. Prior to the scan, the MRI experience was fully explained to the children and their parents. The scanning session took ∼45 min.

fMRI data acquisition

Data were acquired with a 3-Tesla Siemens Allegra scanner. Each scan consisted of one anatomical and two 6 min 36 s functional sequences. T1-weighted 3D MPRAGE anatomic images were acquired [time to repetition (TR)/time to echo (TE) = 23/4 ms, flip angle = 8°, field of view = 256 mm, matrix = 256 × 192 and slice thickness = 1 mm]. Gradient-echo blood-oxygen-level-dependent scans were acquired in 43 contiguous axial slices at a 40° angle to the anterior commissure-posterior commissure (AC–PC) line [TR/TE = 3000/30 ms, slice thickness = 3 mm (0.5 mm skip), in-plane resolution = 3 × 3 mm, 130 data points]. To optimize signal in ventromedial prefrontal regions, the susceptibility artifact was addressed in two ways: (i) acquiring the slices at a 40° angle to the AC–PC line and (ii) positioning all participants in the scanner so that the angle of the AC–PC plane was between 17° and 22° from the axial plane in scanner coordinate space. This procedure also standardized head positioning.

Experimental paradigm

A block design with two functional runs (each run was 6 min 36 s) was used to display the food logos, non-food logos and blurred baseline images (Bruce et al., 2010). Each logo was presented only once to each participant. Functional scans involved three repetitions of each block of stimulus type (i.e. each block contained 10 food logos or 10 non-food logos), alternated between blocks of 10 blurred images. Stimulus presentation time was 2.5 s with an interstimulus interval of 0.5 s. The order of category presentation was counterbalanced across participants. Visual images were back-projected to a screen mounted on the back of the MRI scanner, and participants viewed the images through a mirror on the head coil. Foam cushions were placed around the participants’ heads to minimize movement.

fMRI data analysis

Data preprocessing and statistical analyses were conducted using BrainVoyager QX 2.1 statistical package (Brain Innovation, Maastricht, the Netherlands). The preprocessing steps included trilinear 3D motion correction, sinc-interpolated slice scan time correction, 2D spatial smoothing (4 mm Gaussian filter) and high-pass filter temporal smoothing. Functional images were realigned to the anatomic images obtained within each session and normalized to the BrainVoyager template image, which conforms to the space defined by the Talairach and Tournoux stereotaxic atlas (Talairach and Tournoux, 1988). Four runs out of 34 (two runs each from 17 participants) were discarded due to motion >3 mm of movement on an axis (x, y and z).

Activation maps were generated using statistical parametric methods and random effects in BrainVoyager QX. Statistical contrasts were conducted using multiple regression analysis with the general linear model allowing multiple predictors to be built into the model. Regressors representing experimental conditions of interest were modeled with a hemodynamic response filter and entered into the multiple regression analysis using a random-effects model. Contrasts between conditions of interest were assessed with t-statistics across whole brain. For each contrast (food logo vs baseline, non-food logo vs baseline and food logo vs non-food logo), voxel values were considered significant if the activation survived a statistical cluster-based threshold of P < 0.01, corrected. We corrected for multiple comparisons using the familywise approach (α < 0.05; P < 0.01, k = 9 voxels), determined by Monte Carlo simulation in BrainVoyager (Goebel et al., 2006; Lieberman and Cunningham, 2009).


RESULTS
Food logos vs baseline

As shown in Table 1, the food logos vs baseline analysis (P < 0.01, corrected) revealed significant activations in left OFC [Brodmann’s area (BA) 10/11] (Figure 2) and bilateral inferior frontal gyrus (IFG, BA 13), left temporal cortex and bilateral visual cortex. Significant deactivations to food logos were found in right parietal, bilateral temporal and left posterior cingulate.

Non-food logos vs baseline

The non-food logos vs baseline analysis (P < 0.01, corrected) revealed significant activations in left medial PFC, left IFG, right thalamus and bilateral fusiform gyrus (Table 1). Significant deactivations to non-food logos were found in right superior frontal gyrus, left insula/temporal cortex, bilateral parietal cortex, right temporal cortex and right precuneus.

Food logos vs non-food logos

The food logos vs non-food logos analysis (P < 0.01, corrected) revealed significant activations in right occipital cortex and right paracentral lobule and left parietal and left lingual gyrus (Table 2). Activation in right paracentral lobule extended into posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) (P < 0.01; x = 9, y = −23, z = 43) (Figure 3). No regions showed significantly greater activations to non-food compared with food logos.


DISCUSSION

Although a growing body of neuroimaging literature documents adult brain responses to product brands, this is the first study to examine children’s brain responses to culturally familiar food and non-food logos. In healthy children, food and non-food logos activated object identification regions of the brain (visual cortex/ventral stream). Studies examining adults’ brain responses to logos also noted significant activation in these regions (Plassman et al., 2012).

We found that healthy children’s brains show significant activation to food logos compared to baseline images in regions associated with both motivational value (OFC, BA 10/11) and cognitive control (IFG, BA 13). The non-food logos compared to baseline activated inferior frontal and medial PFC and thalamus. In a direct comparison between food logos and non-food logos, food logos resulted in greater activation in occipital and parietal cortex and PCC. PCC was significantly deactivated to food logos and non-food logos, only more so to non-food logos. PCC is known to be an integral member in the default mode network (Fransson and Marrelec, 2008) and it is possible the deactivations in PCC may indicate the children’s engagement with the visual stimuli. Furthermore, the food logos showed significant positive activations in occipital cortex compared with non-food logos. Other studies have shown that food images elicit brain activations in visual cortex (Simmons et al., 2005). No areas were significantly more active to non-food logos vs food logos. Food logos may attract children’s attention more than non-food logos. This is significant considering the vast majority of foods marketed to children are for unhealthy, calorically dense foods (Powell et al., 2007). Results from this preliminary study should be interpreted using the usual caution pertinent to reverse inference, and may serve as the basis for future hypothesis testing. Researchers should directly compare neural responses to food logos compared to actual images of food.

Our results in children overlap partially with findings from previous studies examining healthy adults’ brain responses to logos including significant activations in medial PFC, inferior PFC, OFC and visual cortex (Plassman et al., 2012). However, unlike the adult studies, we did not observe significant activations in hippocampus or ventral striatum (caudate, nucleus accumbens). Our results are consistent with those found by previous neuroimaging studies examining children’s brain activation in response to actual food images that show activation in PFC and OFC (Holsen et al., 2005; Bruce et al., 2010; Davids et al., 2010).

As this is the first study to examine children’s brain responses to brands, there are some limitations of the results. First, our sample size is relatively small. Future studies with larger samples would permit examination of age and gender effects in response to brands. Second, our study was limited to healthy children and the effects of advertising on obese children were not examined. Given that children are exposed to unhealthy food more often than healthy food (Klepp et al., 2007), such advertising effects may have implications for childhood obesity. Research should examine the differences between healthy weight and obese children’s responses to brands. Third, because we needed to match the food and non-food logos on familiarity, valence and intensity, the logos we chose for the imaging paradigm were not the most familiar, most positively valenced food logos. Thus, findings may underemphasize the effects of food logos on children’s brain responses. Future studies wishing to further clarify the relationship between brain responses to food logos and children’s perceptions of those logos could ask participants to rate the logos while in the scanner. Finally, because we asked participants to refrain from eating for 4 h before the scan to standardize hunger, it is possible that the observed differences in brain activations between food and non-food logos could be due to hunger. Future research should consider manipulating hunger levels of participants to determine whether there is a relationship between brain responses to food logos and food motivation.

From a developmental perspective, these early findings are important, as the brain regions involved in food motivation, reward processing, decision making and self-control change throughout childhood and adolescence (Bruce et al., 2011). A recently published study examined decision making in an intertemporal choice task ($20 now vs $50 in 10 days) using fMRI in conjunction with brand exposure. When a brand logo was subliminally presented to adults before making their choices, preferences shifted to a more immediate reward (Murawski et al., 2012). The prospect of brand exposure altering decision making even in an unrelated task is compelling and worthy of further investigation. Future studies should directly compare youth of different ages and adults to determine how differential maturity affects responses to marketing and decision making regarding food and non-food products.

Children’s brains show responses to brand logos in similar regions as adults’ brains. Food logos, however, seem to be more emotionally salient than the non-food logos, perhaps due to the survival salience of food as a biological necessity. Additional research is needed to better characterize children’s brain responses to marketing and marketing’s impact on their choices and behavior.


SUPPLEMENTARY DATA

Supplementary data are available at SCAN online.


Conflict of Interest

None declared.



This study was supported by the University of Kansas Medical Center Research Institute’s Clinical Pilot Program. The Hoglund Brain Imaging Center is supported by a generous gift from Forrest and Sally Hoglund and by the National Institutes of Health (HD002528 and AG035982). All trademarks in the article are the property of their respective owners.


REFERENCES
Bruce AS,Holsen LM,Chambers RJ,et al. Obese children show hyperactivation to food pictures in brain networks linked to motivation and rewardInternational Journal of ObesityYear: 201034149450020440296
Bruce AS,Martin LE,Savage CR. Neural correlates of pediatric obesityPreventive MedicineYear: 201152Suppl. 1S293521291906
Casey BJ,Giedd JN,Thomas KM. Structural and functional brain development and its relation to cognitive developmentBiological PsychologyYear: 2000541–32415711035225
Coon KA,Tucker KL. Television and children’s consumption patterns. A review of the literatureMinerva PediatricaYear: 2002544233612244280
Davids S,Lauffer H,Thoms K,et al. Increased dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activation in obese children during observation of food stimuliInternational Journal of ObesityYear: 2010349410419806158
DelParigi A,Chen K,Salbe AD,Reiman EM,Tataranni PA. Sensory experience of food and obesity: a positron emission tomography study of the brain regions affected by tasting a liquid meal after a prolonged fastNeuroImageYear: 2005244364315627585
Esch FR,Moll R,Schmitt B,Elger CE,Neuhaus C,Weber B. Brands on the brain: do consumers use declarative information or experienced emotions to evaluate brands?Journal of Consumer PsychologyYear: 2012227585
Federal Trade CommissionChildren’s Exposure to TV Advertising in 1977 and 2004. Bureau of Economics Staff ReportYear: 2007Washington, DCFederal Trade Commission
Fransson P,Marrelec G. The precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex plays a pivotal role in the default mode network: evidence from a partial correlation network analysisNeuroImageYear: 20084211788418598773
Gautier JF,Chen K,Salbe AD,et al. Differential brain responses to satiation in obese and lean menDiabetesYear: 20004958384610905495
Goebel R,Espozito F,Formisano ER,Espozito F,Formisano E. Analysis of Functional Image Analysis Contest (FIAC) data with Brain Voyager QX: from single-subject to cortically aligned group general linear model analysis and self-organizing group independent component analysisHuman Brain MappingYear: 20062739240116596654
Harris JL,Bargh JA,Brownell KD. Priming effects of television food advertising on eating behaviorHealth PsychologyYear: 2009284041319594263
Holsen LM,Zarcone JR,Thompson TI,et al. Neural mechanisms underlying food motivation in children and adolescentsNeuroImageYear: 2005276697615993629
Institute of Medicine Committee on Food Marketing and the Diets of Children and YouthFood Marketing to Children and Youth: Threat or Opportunity?Year: 2006Washington, DCThe National Academies Press
Killgore WD,Yurgelun-Todd DA. Developmental changes in the functional brain responses of adolescents to images of high and low-calorie foodsDevelopmental NeurobiologyYear: 20054737797
Klepp KI,Wind M,de Bourdeaudhuij I,et al. Television viewing and exposure to food-related commercials among European school children, associations with fruit and vegetable intake: a cross sectional studyInternational Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical ActivityYear: 2007446
Lang PJ,Bradley MM,Cuthbert BN. International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Affective Ratings of Pictures and Instruction ManualYear: 2008 Technical Report A-8. University of Florida, Gainesville, FL.
Lieberman MD,Cunningham WA. Type I and type II error concerns in fMRI research: re-balancing the scaleSocial Cognitive and Affective NeuroscienceYear: 20094423820035017
Livingstone S. Assessing the research base for the policy debate over the effects of food advertising to childrenInternational Journal of AdvertisingYear: 200524327396
Martin LE,Holsen LM,Chambers R,et al. Neural mechanisms associated with food motivation in obese and healthy weight adultsObesityYear: 20101822546019629052
McClure SM,Li J,Tomlin D,Cypert KS,Montague LM,Montague PR. Neural correlates of behavior preference for culturally familiar drinksNeuronYear: 2004443798715473974
Murawski C,Harris PG,Bode S,Domingues JF,Egan GF. Led into temptation? Rewarding brand logos bias the neural encoding of incidental economic decisionsPublic Library of Science ONEYear: 201273111
Plassmann H,Ramsoy TZ,Milosavljevic M. Branding the brain: a critical review and outlookJournal of Consumer PsychologyYear: 2012221836
Powell LM,Szczypka G,Chaloupka FJ,Braunschweig CL. Nutritional content of television food advertisements seen by children and adolescents in the United StatesPediatricsYear: 200712035768317766531
Robinson TN,Borzekowski DL,Matheson DM,Kraemer HC. Effects of fast food branding on young children’s taste preferencesArchives of Pediatric and Adolescent MedicineYear: 200716187927
Schaefer M,Berens H,Heinze H,Rotte M. Neural correlates of culturally familiar brands of car manufacturersNeuroImageYear: 200631861516487728
Schaefer M,Rotte M. Thinking on luxury or pragmatic brand products: brain responses to different categories of culturally based brandsBrain ResearchYear: 2007a11659810417655834
Schaefer M,Rotte M. Favorite brands as cultural objects modulate reward circuitNeuroReportYear: 2007b182141517301679
Schaefer M,Knuth M,Rumpel F. Striatal response to favorite brands as a function of neuroticism and extraversionBrain ResearchYear: 2011142583922035566
Simmons WK,Martin A,Barsalou LW. Pictures of appetizing foods activate gustatory cortices for taste and rewardCerebral CortexYear: 2005151602815703257
Small DM,Zatorre RJ,Dagher A,Evans AC,Jones-Gotman M. Changes in brain activity related to eating chocolate: from pleasure to aversionBrainYear: 200112417203311522575
Somerville LH,Casey BJ. Developmental neurobiology of cognitive control and motivational systemsCurrent Opinion in NeurobiologyYear: 2010202364120167473
Talairach J,Tournoux P. Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human BrainYear: 1988New YorkThieme Medical Publishers Inc

Figures

[Figure ID: nss109-F1]
Fig. 1 

Example of item from the pilot validation of logo stimuli prior to the main fMRI study.



[Figure ID: nss109-F2]
Fig. 2 

fMRI statistical maps (sagittal perspective) showing results from food logo vs baseline contrasts, co-registered with average structural MRI data from participants. Significance thresholds are set at P < 0.01, corrected (cluster threshold = 9 voxels). Arrow highlights greater activation in OFC.



[Figure ID: nss109-F3]
Fig. 3 

fMRI statistical maps in the sagittal view showing results from food vs non-food logo contrasts, co-registered with average structural MRI data from participants. Significance thresholds are set at P < 0.01, corrected (cluster threshold = 9 voxels). Arrow highlights greater activation in PCC.



Tables
[TableWrap ID: nss109-T1] Table 1 

Regions reaching significance for the contrasts between food and non-food logo stimuli in comparison to baseline images


Contrast and region Coordinates
t-Value Contiguous voxels
x y z
Food logos vs baseline
    OFC (L), BA 10/11 −6 41 −8 5.96 114
    Bilateral IFG, BA 13 −39 29 10 6.78 280
        BA 47 24 29 −2 6.19 15
 Bilateral occipital cortex, BA 18 27 −82 1 8.43 1326
−27 −79 −11 11.47 1547
 Bilateral temporal cortex −51 −34 1 6.36 11
−57 −16 −17 5.52 12
−48 −22 13 −5.01 30
39 −10 −5 −3.92 12
    Parietal cortex (R), BA 40 63 −37 31 −5.44 90
    Posterior cingulate (L), BA 31 −6 −46 43 −6.02 183
Non-food logos vs baseline
    Medial prefrontal (L), BA 6 −9 5 55 6.82 10
    IFG (L), BA 13 −42 26 10 5.71 119
    Thalamus (R) 21 −25 4 4.82 14
 Bilateral fusiform gyrus, BA 19/37 39 −70 −11 9.88 1328
−39 −46 −17 11.28 1445
 Superior frontal gyrus (R), BA 10/9 30 68 1 −4.21 11
30 59 28 −4.63 11
    Insula (L), BA 13 −42 −10 4 −5.86 63
    Insula/temporal cortex (L) −45 −31 19 −7.01 19
 Precuneus (R), BA 30/31 15 −55 16 −4.60 13
6 −37 43 −5.75 372
    Temporal cortex (R), BA 21 54 −22 −2 −5.44 20
 Bilateral parietal cortex 63 −28 43 −5.38 180
−63 −28 37 −5.05 87

nss109-TF1P < 0.01, cluster corrected at 9 voxels. Activations are listed first (positive t-values) followed by deactivations (negative t-values) for each contrast. L = left; R = right.


[TableWrap ID: nss109-T2] Table 2 

Regions reaching significance for the contrasts food logo stimuli in comparison to non-food logo stimuli


Contrast and region Coordinates
t-Value Contiguous voxels
x y z
Food logos > non-food logos
    Occipital cortex (R), BA 18 18 −85 11 6.23 71
    Lingual gyrus (L), BA 17 −6 −88 −2 5.73 89
    Paracentral/PCC (R), BA 31 9 −28 46 4.41 18
    Parietal cortex (L), BA 40 −24 −40 55 4.85 13

nss109-TF2P < 0.01, cluster corrected at 9 voxels. There were no regions where non-food logos > food logos. L = left; R = right.



Article Categories:
  • Original Articles

Keywords: children, brands, fMRI, prefrontal cortex, neuromarketing, food logos.

Previous Document:  Imaging depletion: fMRI provides new insights into the processes underlying ego depletion*.
Next Document:  Simultaneous preconcentration and determination of 2,4-D, alachlor and atrazine in aqueous samples u...